SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

Referencias del artículo

ROCA, Alicia  y  BRUNER, Carlos A.. Un análisis del origen del consumo excesivo de agua del beber inducido por el programa. Rev. mex. anál. conducta [online]. 2011, vol.37, n.2, pp. 177-204. ISSN 0185-4534.  http://dx.doi.org/10.5514/rmac.v37.i2.26146.

    Allen, J. D., Porter, J. H., & Arazie, R. (1975). Schedule–induced drinking as a function of percentage reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 23, 223–232, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1975.23–223 [ Links ]

    Bolles, R. C. (1961). The interaction of hunger and thirst in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 54, 580–584, available via: http://dx.doi. org/10.1037/h0044595 [ Links ]

    Bruner, C. A., & Ávila, R. (2002). Adquisición y mantenimiento del palanqueo en ratas sin privación explícita del reforzador. Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta, 28, 107–130. [ Links ]

    Brush, M. E., & Schaeffer, R. W. (1974). Effects of water deprivation on schedule–induced polydipsia. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 4, 69–72. [ Links ]

    Christian, W. P. (1975). Effects of the quality of dry food reinforcement on the rat's instrumental licking and drinking behavior. The Psychological Record, 25, 237–242. [ Links ]

    Christian, W. P., Schaeffer, R. W., & King, G. D. (1977). Schedule–induced Behavior. Montreal: Eden. [ Links ]

    Clark, F. C. (1962). Some observations on the adventitious reinforcement of drinking under food reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, 61–63, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1962.5–61 [ Links ]

    Couch, J. V. (1974). Reinforcement magnitude and schedule–induced polydipsia: A reexamination. Psychological Record, 24, 559–562. [ Links ]

    De Castro, J. M. (1989). The interactions of fluid and food intake in the spontaneous feeding and drinking patterns of rats. Physiology and Behavior, 45, 861–870. [ Links ]

    Díaz, F. J., & Bruner, C. A. (2007). Comer y beber en ratas con libre acceso a la comida y al agua. Acta Comportamentalia, 15, 111–130. [ Links ]

    Falk, J. L. (1961). Production of polydipsia in normal rats by an intermittent food schedule. Science, 133, 195–196. [ Links ]

    Falk, J. L. (1964). Studies on schedule–induced polydipsia. En M. J. Wayner (Ed.), Thirst: First International Symposium on Thirst in the Regulation of Body Water (pp. 95–116). Nueva York: Pergamon Press. [ Links ]

    Falk, J. L. (1966b). Schedule–induced Polydipsia as a function of fixed–interval length. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 37–39, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1966.9–37 [ Links ]

    Falk, J. L. (1967). Control of schedule–induced Polydipsia: Type, size, and spacing of meals. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 199–206, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1967.10–199 [ Links ]

    Falk, J. L. (1969). Conditions producing psychogenic polydipsia in animals. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 157, 569–593, available via: http://dx.doi. org/10.1111/j.1749–6632.1969.tb12908.x [ Links ]

    Falk, J. L. (1971). The nature and determinants of adjunctive behavior. Physiology and Behavior, 6, 577–588, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0031–9384(71)90209–5 [ Links ]

    Falk, J. L. (1977). The origin and functions of adjunctive behavior. Animal Learning and Behavior, 5, 325–335, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03209574 [ Links ]

    Fitzsimons, T. J., & Le Magnen, J. (1969). Eating as a regulatory control of drinking in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 67, 273–283, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0026772 [ Links ]

    Flory, R. K. (1971). The control of schedule–induced polydipsia: Frequency and magnitude of reinforcement. Learning and Motivation, 2, 215–227, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0023–9690(71)90022–1 [ Links ]

    Freed, E., & Hymowitz, N. (1972). Effects of schedule, percent body weight, and magnitude of reinforcer on the acquisition of schedule–induced polydipsia. Psychological Reports, 31, 95–101. [ Links ]

    Green, L., & Freed, D. E. (1993). The substitutability of reinforcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 141–158, available via: http://dx.doi. org/10.1901/jeab.1993.60–141 [ Links ]

    Green, L., & Rachlin, H. (1991). Economic substitutability of electrical brain stimulation, food, and water. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55, 133–143, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1991.55–133 [ Links ]

    Hursh, S. R. (1978). The economics of daily consumption controlling food– and water reinforced responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 29, 475–491, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1978.29–475 [ Links ]

    King, G. D., Mc Gill, D., Pierson, S. C., & Schaeffer, R. W. (1972). Schedule–induced alcohol and water intakes in rats on a FFI 60 sec. schedule. Psychological Reports, 30, 291–296. [ Links ]

    López–Espinoza, A., & Martínez, H. (2005). Efectos de intervalos variables entre períodos de privación sobre el consumo post–privación de agua y comida en ratas. Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta, 31, 67–84, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.5514/rmac.v31.i1.23241 [ Links ]

    Lotter, E. C., Woods, S. C., & Vasseli, J. R. (1973). Schedule–induced polydipsia: An artifact. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 83, 478–484, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0034670 [ Links ]

    Madden, G.J., Smethells,J. R., Ewan, E. E., & Hursh, S. R. (2007). Tests of behavioral–economic assessments of relative reinforcer efficacy II: Economic complements. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 88, 355–367, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2007.88–355 [ Links ]

    Pellón, R., & Blackman, D. E. (1987). Punishment of Schedule–induced drinking in rats by signaled and unsignaled delays in food presentation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 48, 417–434, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1987.48–417 [ Links ]

    Reid, A. K., & Staddon, J. E. R. (1982). Schedule–induced drinking: Elicitation, anticipation or behavioral interaction? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 38, 1–18, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1982.38–1 [ Links ]

    Robinson, J. K., & Flory, R. K. (1989). The control of schedule–induced polydipsia by meal size: Interaction with prior within–session eating and drinking. The Psychological Record, 39, 439–445. [ Links ]

    Roca, A., & Bruner, C. A. (2003). Efectos de la frecuencia de reforzamiento sobre el palanqueo en ratas privadas de comida. Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta, 29, 119–130. [ Links ]

    Roca, A., & Bruner, C. A. (2007). Intromisión de una sesión de beber inducido por el programa en un período de 24 horas. Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta, 33, 61–78, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.5514/rmac.v33.i1.16267 [ Links ]

    Roper, T. J., & Nieto, J. (1979). Schedule–induced drinking and other behavior in the rat, as a function of body weight deficit. Physiology and Behavior, 23, 673–678, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0031–9384(79)90159–8 [ Links ]

    Rosenblith, J. Z. (1970). Polydipsia induced in the rat by a second–order schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 14, 139–144, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1970.14–139 [ Links ]

    Ruiz, J. A., & Bruner, C. A. (2008). Demora de reforzamiento con agua en un procedimiento de beber inducido por el programa. Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta, 34, 97–109, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.5514/rmac.v34.i1.16236 [ Links ]

    Segal, E. F., Oden, D. L., & Deadwyler, S. A. (1965). Determinants of polydipsia: IV. Free reinforcement schedules. Psychonomic Science, 3, 11–12. [ Links ]

    Staddon, J. E. R. (1977). Schedule–Induced Behavior. En W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of Operant Behavior (pp. 125–152). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice–Hall. [ Links ]

    Stein, L. (1964). Excessive drinking in the rat: Superstition or thirst? Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 58, 237–242. [ Links ]

    Timberlake, W. (1997). An animal–centered, causal system approach to the understanding and control of behavior. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 53, 107–129, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0168–1591(96)01154–9 [ Links ]

    Timberlake, W. (2000). Motivational modes in behavior systems. En R. R. Mowrer & S. B. Klein (Eds.), Handbook of Contemporary Learning (pp. 155–209). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates. [ Links ]

    Verplanck, W. S., & Hayes, J. R. (1953). Eating and drinking as a function of maintenance schedule. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47, 327– 333, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0055380 [ Links ]

    Wetherington, C. L. (1979). Schedule–induced drinking: Rate of food delivery and Herrnstein's equation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 32, 323–333, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1979.32–323 [ Links ]

    Wetherington, C. L. (1982). Is adjunctive behavior a third class of behavior? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 6, 329–350, available via: http://dx.doi. org/10.1016/0149–7634(82)90045–8 [ Links ]