SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

Referencias del artículo

DELGADO, Diana; MEDINA, Iván Felipe  y  SOTO, Joan Sebastián. El lenguaje como mediador de la transferencia de funciones: ¿es necesario nominar para relacionar?. Rev. mex. anál. conducta [online]. 2011, vol.37, n.2, pp. 31-52. ISSN 0185-4534.  http://dx.doi.org/10.5514/rmac.v37.i2.26138.

    Anderson, N. D., Craik, F. I. M., & Naveh-Benjamin, M. (1998). The attentional demands of encoding and retrieval in younger and older adults: I. Evidence from divided attention costs. Psychology and Aging, 13(3), 405-423, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.13.3.405. [ Links ]

    Barnes, D., McCullagh, P. D., & Keenan, M. (1990). Equivalence class formation in non-hearing impaired children and hearing impaired children. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 8, 19-30 [ Links ]

    Carr, D., Wikinson, K. M., Blackman, D., & McIlvane, W. J. (2000). Equivalence classes in individuals with minimal verbal repertoires. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74(1), 101-114, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/ jeab.2000.74-101 [ Links ]

    Chaparro, J. (2010) Efectos de diferentes condiciones de retroalimentación sobre la transformación de funciones. Tesis de Grado no publicada. Fundación Universitaria Konrad Lorenz. [ Links ]

    Clayton, M. C., & Hayes, L. J. (2004). A comparison of match to sample and respondent-type training of equivalence classes. The Psychological Record, 54, 579-602. [ Links ]

    Craik, F. I. M., Govoni, R., Naveh-Benjamin, M., & Anderson, N. D. (1996). The effect of divided attention on encoding and retrieval processes in human memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125(2), 159-180, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.125.2.159. [ Links ]

    Delgado, D. (2005). Substitution of stimulus functions as a means to distinguish among different types of functional classes. Tesis de maestría no publicada. University of Nevada, Reno, Reno, NV. [ Links ]

    Delgado, D., & Hayes, L. (2011) Using the operant MTS procedure as masking task for respondent acquisition of stimulus clases. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 43(1), 13-24. [ Links ]

    Delgado, D., & Medina, I. (2011). Efectos de dos tipos de entrenamiento respondiente sobre la formación de relaciones de equivalencia. Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta, 37(1). doi:10.5514/rmac.v37.i1.19475 [ Links ]

    Dugdale, N., & Lowe, C. F. (1990). Naming and stimulus equivalence. En. D. E. Blackman & H. Lejeune (Eds.) Behavior Analysis in Theory and Practice: Contributions and Controversies., pp 115-138, England: Erlbaum. [ Links ]

    Fernandez, M. A., & Moscovitch, M. (2000). Divided attention and memory: Evidence of substantial interference effects at retrieval and encoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(2), 155-176, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.129.2.155 [ Links ]

    Fields, L. (1996). The evidence of naming as a cause or facilitator of equivalence class formation. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65(1), 279-281, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1996.65-279. [ Links ]

    Gutiérrez, M. T., & Benjumea, S. (2003). Formación de clases funcionales utilizando un entrenamiento en condicionamiento clásico. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 35(2), 165-174 [ Links ]

    Goyos, C. (1996). Comportamiento verbal y equivalencia. Psicothema, 8(1), 119-131. [ Links ]

    Hayes, L. J. (1992). Equivalence as a process. En: Hayes, S. C. & L. J. Hayes (Eds.), Understanding Verbal Relations. pp, 97-108, Reno: Context Press. [ Links ]

    Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational Frame Theory. A Post-Skinnerian Account of Human Language and Cognition. NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. [ Links ]

    Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J. (1992). Verbal relations and the evolution of the behavior analysis. American Psychologist, 47(11), 1383-1395, available via: http://dx.doi. org/10.1037/0003-066X.47.11.1383 [ Links ]

    Horne, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185-241, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1996.65-185 [ Links ]

    Horne, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1997). Toward a theory of verbal behavior. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 68(2), 271-296, available via: http://dx.doi. org/10.1901/jeab.1997.68-271. [ Links ]

    Johnston, W. A., & Heinz, S. P (1978) Flexibility and capacity demands of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 107(4), 420-435, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.107.4.420 [ Links ]

    Kastak, C. R., Schusterman, R. J., & Kastak, D. (2001). Equivalence classifications by California sea lions using class specific reinforcers. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 76(2), 131-158, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/ jeab.2001.76-131 [ Links ]

    Leader, G., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2001). Matching to sample and respondent-type training as methods for producing equivalence relations: isolating the critical variable. The Psychological Record, 51, 429-444. [ Links ]

    Leader, G., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smeets, P.M. (1996) Establishing equivalence relations using a respondent-type training procedure. The Psychological Record, 46, 685-706. [ Links ]

    Leader, G., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Smeets P. M. (2000). Establishing equivalence relations using a respondent-type training procedure III. The Psychological Record, 50, 63-78. [ Links ]

    Lee, V. L., & Pegler, A. M. (1982). Effects on spelling of training children to read. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 311-322, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1982.37-311. [ Links ]

    Lionello De-Nolf, K. M. (2009). The search for symmetry 25 years in review. Learning & Behavior, 37(2), 188-203. [ Links ]

    Logan, G. D., & Burkell. J. (1986). Dependence and independence in responding to double stimulation: A comparison of stop, change and dual-task paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12, 549-563, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.12.4.549 [ Links ]

    Luciano, C. (1993). La conducta verbal a la luz de recientes investigaciones. Su papel sobre otras conductas verbales y no verbales. Psicothema, 5(2), 351-374. [ Links ]

    Martin, J. A. (1971). The control of imitative and nonimitative behaviors in severely retarded children though "generalized-instruction following. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 11, 390-400, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(71)90044-0 [ Links ]

    Moscovitch, M. (1994). Cognitive resources and DA interference effects at retrieval in normal people: The role of frontal lobes and medial temporal cortex. Neuropsychology, 8, 524-534, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.8.4.524 [ Links ]

    Nakagawa, E. (2001). Acquired equivalence of cues in learning a matching-to-sample task by rats. The Psychological Record, 51(3), 453-472. [ Links ]

    Naveh-Benjamin, M., Craik, F. I. M., Gavrilescu, D., & Anderson, N. D. (2000). Asymmetry between encoding and retrieval processes: Evidence from a divided attention paradigm and a calibration analysis. Memory & Cognition, 28(6), 965-976, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03209344 [ Links ]

    Naveh-Benjamin, M., Craik, F. I. M., Guez, J., & Dori, H. (1998). Effects of divided attention on encoding and retrieval processes in human memory: Further support for an asymmetry. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(5), 1091-1104, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.24.5.1091 [ Links ]

    Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2002). Implicit acquisition and manifestation of classically conditioned attitudes. Social Cognition, 20(2), 89-103, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.20.2.89.20992. [ Links ]

    Page, M. M. (1964). Demand characteristics and the social psychology of classical conditioning attitudes experiment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 177-186. [ Links ]

    Page, M. M. (1974). Social psychology of classical conditioning attitudes experiment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11(2), 468-476. [ Links ]

    Pashler, H (1994). Dual-Task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116(2), 220-244, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.220 [ Links ]

    Peña, T., Pitts, R., & Galizio, M. (2006). Identity matching-to-sample with olfatory stimuli in rats. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 85(2), 203-221, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2006.111-04. [ Links ]

    Rehfeldt, R. A., & Barnes-Homes, Y. (2009). Derived Relational Responding. Applications for Learners with Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities. Oakland: New Harbinger Publications. [ Links ]

    Rehfeldt, R. A., & Hayes, L. J. (1998). The operant-respondent extinction revisited: Toward an understanding of stimulus equivalence. The Psychological Record, 48, 187. [ Links ]

    Sidman, M. (1992). Equivalence Relations: Some Basic Considerations. En: S. C. Hayes, & Hayes, L. J. (Eds.). Understanding Verbal Relations. pp 15-26. Reno, NV: Context Press. [ Links ]

    Sidman, M. (2000). Equivalence relations and the reinforcement contingency. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74(1), 127-146, available via: http://dx.doi. org/10.1901/jeab.2000.74-127 [ Links ]

    Smeets, P. M., Leader, G., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (1997). Establishing stimulus classes in adults and children using a respondent-type training procedure: A follow-up study. The Psychological Record, 47(2), 285-308. [ Links ]

    Tonneau, F. (2001). Equivalence relations: a critical analysis. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 2(1), 1-33. [ Links ]

    Tonneau, F., Arreola, F., & Martínez, A. (2006). Function transformation without reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 85(3), 393-405, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2006.49-05 [ Links ]

    Tonneau, F., & González, C. (2004). Function transfer in human operant experiments: The role of stimulus pairings. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 81(3), 239-255, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2004.81-239 [ Links ]

    Urcuioli, P. J. (2008). Associative symmetry, antisymmetry and a theory of pigeons' equivalence class formation. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 90(3), 257-282, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2008.90-257. [ Links ]

    Vaughan, W. (1988). Formation of equivalence sets in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 14, 36-42, available via: http://dx.doi. org/10.1037/0097-7403.14.1.36 [ Links ]

    Walther, E. (2002). Guilty by mere association: Evaluative conditioning and the spreading attitude effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 919- 934, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.919 [ Links ]

    Wilson, K., & Hayes, S. (1996) Resurgence of derived stimulus relations. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 66(3), 267-281, available via: http://dx.doi. org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.919 [ Links ]

    Wulfert, E., Dougher, M. J., & Greenway, D. E. (1991). Protocol analysis of the correspondence of verbal behavior and equivalence class formation. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56(3), 489-504. [ Links ]

    Zentall, T. R., Sherburne, L. M., & Steirn, J. N. (1992). Development of excitatory backward associations Turing the establishment of forward associations in a delayed condicional discrimination by pigeons. Animal Learning & Behavior, 20, 199-206. [ Links ]