SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.5 número2Las variedades de español según los hispanohablantes: corrección, incorrección y agrado lingüísticosTipos de predicación metonímica en la composición nominal del español de México índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Revista

Articulo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • No hay artículos similaresSimilares en SciELO

Compartir


Cuadernos de Lingüística de El Colegio de México

versión On-line ISSN 2007-736X

Cuad. Lingüíst. Col. Méx. vol.5 no.2 Ciudad de México jul./dic. 2018

https://doi.org/10.24201/clecm.v5i2.116 

Artículos

Linguistic Geography of Huastec (teːnek)

Geografía lingüística del idioma huasteco (teːnek)

Lucero Meléndez Guadarramaa 

aInstituto de Investigaciones Antropológicas. Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México. luceromelendezg@gmail.com


Abstract

The present paper aims to show current Huastec (teːnek) linguistic variation and its geographical distribution based on phonological, morphological, lexical and syntactic features. First, it covers the framework known as linguistic geography (geolinguistics) and describes the methodology employed in this study, which is primarily descriptive. Secondly, it presents a discussion regarding previous Huastec dialectal groupings, and the need to go further into the study of its variations. The third part shows several maps that display the variations of the isoglosses, detailing the Western and Eastern dialects' synchronic variation, as well as the linguistic data that support them. In the last section, the research focuses on the Central dialect (Tantoyuca), which has evolved independently from the other dialects at the phonological level, but which clearly shares morphological features with the Western dialect. These features are highly relevant in the grammar of the language, and vital to trace the recent history of the region.

Keywords: Huastec dialectology; Huastec historical linguistics; phonological variation; lexical variation; morphological variation

1. Introduction

Huastec is a Mayan language spoken since pre-Hispanic times in the Huasteca region, far away from the rest of its linguistic family located in the "Mayan Region".1 The auto-denomination of Huastec language is teːnek, but this term is not used for practical reasons since "Huastec" is the name used in the academic field. It is spoken by an estimate of 167,000 people in the states of Veracruz and San Luis Potosí, Mexico, and although the absolute number could suggest that it is not endangered, most of the speakers in Veracruz are only adults and elderly people. Huastec is one of the Mayan Languages that have not been properly studied, and it also lacks pre-Hispanic written records. The oldest documents are three linguistic registers from Colonial times, which show the clear dialectal differences that existed among them.

In the Catalogue of National Indigenous Languages2 (2008), published by the National Institute of Indigenous Languages,3 it has been settled that Huastec has three dialectal variants, namely: Western or Potosino, Central (Tantoyuca region, Veracruz) and Eastern (Sierra de Otontepec, Veracruz), geographically distributed as shown in Map 1.

Map 1 Geographical Distribution of the Three Huastec Dialects According to CLIN (2008). Map by Perla Pisson Alcaraz (2016), based on data from National Institute of Statistics and Geography (henceforth, INEGI) 

The idea to review the Huastec dialectal grouping presented by Kaufman (1985) arose after studying the morphological data of the person system that revealed an existing variation (Meléndez 2011, 2017). The overlap and divergence of isoglosses needed to be explained in order to describe the intra-dialectal variation attested in Eastern Huastec. The research focuses on the fact that some morphological features regarding the person system tend to overlap isoglosses in a regular way in the Central and Potosino dialects (see Map 2). This is an interesting fact when contrasted with the phonological level, in which varieties are clearly differentiated.

Map 2 Isoglosses of Person System (partially modified from Meléndez 2011: 242

Besides the historical relevance of explaining the morphological tendency to group Central and Western dialects together, leading a specific intra-dialectal study in the Eastern dialect becomes even more relevant since it is a hugely endangered variety (Embriz and Zamora 2012: 36). Also, the Eastern dialect has barely been described: only one of the communities has been studied, but the rest are far from being documented (see San Francisco Chontla Grammar, Kondic 2012).

In view of the factors listed above, the present research shows new comparative work in order to reveal the features that overlap and those that differ among dialects and the preliminary results are presented as maps. 4 The intention of this research is not to propose a new dialectal grouping, but to reveal the current diversification in the Huastec language. These findings could be the basis for more academic research either in the field of dialectology or in the linguistic documentation of the variants, especially those that are seriously threatened and had not been yet described or documented. 5

2. Framework and Methodology

The framework of geolinguistics, or linguistic geography, is related to dialectology but instead of studying the dialectal grouping of a language using only phonology, it also deals with morphological and lexical variations in spoken languages. Geolinguistics also contemplates the factor of geographical space as one of the most important aspects to determine the linguistic variation within a specific language. Using this approach, data are obtained by semi-structured interviews and the variation data are organized into maps in order to create a linguistic atlas of a language. According to García Mouton (1996: 63), linguistic geography is not a science by itself, but a useful tool to study variations within spoken language. As Coseriu (1991: 102) states, linguistic geography is “un método dialectológico y comparativo [...] que presupone el registro en mapas especiales de un número relativamente elevado de formas lingüísticas (fónicas, léxicas o gramaticales) comprobadas mediante encuesta directa y unitaria en una red de puntos de un territorio determinado” [“A dialectological and comparative method […] presupposing the register, in special maps, of a relatively high number of linguistic forms (phonic, lexical, or grammatical) tested throughout a network of places in a determined territory through direct, unitary surveys”].

The survey net ("red de encuesta") for this study was collected during three different stages in nine Huastec communities, which cover almost the entire region where Huastec is currently spoken (see Table 1 and Map 3 for geographical location).

Table 1 Communities Selected for this Study 

Name of the Huastec communities State
1. San Nicolás, Tanlajás SLP
2. Mantetzulel, Aquismón SLP
3. Tamaletom, Tancanhuitz SLP
4. San Antonio Huitzquilico, Xilitla SLP
5.Francisco Villa, San Vicente Tancuayalab SLP
6. El Mamey, San Gabriel, Tantoyuca Veracruz
7. Tancoco, Tancoco Veracruz
8. San Francisco, Chontla Veracruz
9. Chinampa de Gorostiza Veracruz

Map 3 Geographic Location of the Selected Communities. Map by Oscar Zamora. 

The criteria to select these communities are: 1) that they have consistently shown some degree of grammatical or lexical variation with respect to each other in previous studies or, 2) guided by perceptual dialectology, that these are the areas with respect to which speakers declared to notice some kind of variation. The sample included one man and one woman from each of the nine communities, and although it was preferred that those speakers were between 35 and 50 years old, in some cases the only speakers left on the community were over sixty years old and the survey was applied to them. Regarding the questionnaires, two different lists were adapted and used:

  • a) Terrence Kaufman's list (ms.) for the phonological and lexical data, with a total of 460 entries. This tool was formerly designed to describe the dialectal variation of the Mayan languages in Guatemala in the early 70s.

  • b) Thomas Smith's list (ms.) for morphological and simple clauses, with a total of 435 entries.

3. Previous Research on Huastec Dialectology

Since Colonial times, friars noticed the Huastec dialectal variation and added a few notes in their texts; (see de la Cruz 1571: f.5) "y ansi para doctrina bre-ue no resta mas para que se enseñe con bendicion en toda la guasteca, pues de todos los pueblos della se tomo lo mas polido y conueniente de que vuestra señoria Reuerendissima la admita en seruicio" [And so, for a brief doctrine, nothing else but your Highness’ acceptance is necessary to teach it, with blessings, throughout the whole Huasteca region, because the most polished and convenient elements [of this language] were taken from each town”]

Tapia Zenteno (1767: 2) also claimed that:

Tendrá gran cuidado el principiante en no tropezar en el dialecto y pronunciación de los de Tamtoyoc, y la mayor parte de la jurisdicción de Tampico, excepto Panoco, que no la pronuncian como está dicho, mayormente los Serranos de Tamtima, sino que en lugar de la tz, usan en su lugar de ch pronunciandola como nosotros en castellano, y afectando un remilgo... [Beginners must be cautious not to stumble with the dialect and pronunciation of the Tamtoyoc people and most of those who live in the Tampico jurisdiction-with the exception of Panoco-who do not pronounce it as said, particularly those who live in the Tamtina Sierra; instead of the ‘tz,’ they pronounce the ‘ch’ as we do in Spanish…]

In her grammatical description of Xiloxúchil (Veracruz) Huastec, Ochoa (1984) concludes that Huastec has two dialects: Potosino and Veracruzano. Later, in 2007, Ochoa wrote some notes about Huastec dialectology and she suggested a new subdivision of the Veracruzano dialect into two "sub-dialects", i.e. Lomerío, and Serrano u Otontepec. She claimed that they were not different dialects; but, based on mutual intelligibility, they could only be considered as "sub-dialects."

Map 4 Huastec Dialects Based on Ochoa (1984; 2007). Map by Oscar Zamora based on INEGI 2010 and CLIN 2008. 

The first formal attempt to propose a dialectal study was Kaufman's brief article “Aspects of Huastec Dialectology and Historical Phonology” (1985). Based on the historical evolution of three phonemes summarized in Figure 1, he proposed that Huastec has three dialectal variants: Eastern, Central and Western6.

Figure 1 Evolution of the three phonological traits considered by Kaufman (1985:474-5)  

Kaufman's paper proposed that these three "non-uniform phonological traits found in Huastec" reveal the internal relations among the Huastecan languages. Kaufman concludes that Huastec dialects differentiated very recently (around 400 years ago), since most of the loanwords from Nahuatl and Spanish into Huastec show evidence of recent evolution in the different dialects. One of Kaufman’s most important contribution was the "official"recognition of the Eastern Huastec as a well-differentiated dialect, which until then had been seen as part of a bigger single dialect called "Veracruzano".

Map 5 Distribution of Huastec Dialects Based on Kaufman (1985). Map by Oscar Zamora based on INEGI 2010 and CLIN 2008. 

In 2008, INALI published the Catálogo de las Lenguas Indígenas Nacionales [Catalogue of National Indigenous Languages], where it was officially stated that Huastec features the three dialects proposed by Kaufman (1985). The geographical distribution of these three dialects is shown in Maps 4 and 5, with different color dots: purple for the Western dialect, blue for Central dialect and red for the Eastern dialect. Two years later, Meléndez (2011, 2017) noticed that the Eastern dialect presented morphological variation that could lead to a new subdivision: Tancoco speakers (#7) and San Francisco Chontla (#8) speakers differentiated in various morphological features. On the other hand, she observed that Central and Western dialects share recent innovations in the morphological level and this issue needed to be explained since they clearly differ in the phonological level.

Phonological features are the most common criteria to propose a dialectal grouping, since phonology is highly regular. Nevertheless, new attempts to include other grammatical levels have shown more variation than previously thought, leading to a finer view of a particular type of language diversity. In Huastec, the best-known isoglosses relate with the evolution of the proto-Huastec phoneme *c into the different current variants. This diversification was noticed during the 18th century by Tapia Zenteno (1767), and later described by Ochoa (2007) and Kaufman (1985) in their work (see Map 6 for the reflections of this phoneme in the different Huastec dialects).

Map 6 Reflections of Proto-Huastec */c/. Map by Oscar Zamora. 

The second phonological isogloss is related to the evolution of the affricate retroflex */t̥/, as shown in Map 7.

Map 7 Reflections of Proto-Huastec */t̥/. Map by Oscar Zamora. 

And, finally, the isogloss that refers to the evolution of the proto phoneme */b'/, as exhibited in Map 8:

Map 8 Reflections of Proto-Huastec */b'/. Map by Oscar Zamora. 

As Kaufman (1985) realized, Chontla seems to be very peculiar in its linguistic development compared with the rest of the dialects, including other communities located in the Sierra de Otontepec region. After an analysis of the particularities that Kaufman found in Chontla, and the author’s phonological research, some variations were found between Tancoco (#7), Chinampa de Gorostiza (#9) and Chontla (#8). In other words, Eastern Huastec does not behave as a regular dialectal unit, and this diversification needs to be explored in further studies.

Beyond phonology, and in the light of the variation attested in morphological, lexical and syntactical levels, it is necessary to perform an intensive research to describe current Huastec variations. In order to clarify the scope of this variation, some research was done from 2014 to 2016 in the project "Documentación lingüística del Huasteco." Some of its preliminary data are presented in this paper; nevertheless, results are not conclusive. This information is shared in order to encourage more sociolinguistic studies, so as to find a more accurate perspective on Huastec dialectology.

4. Eastern Huastec New Isoglosses

4.1 The phoneme /l/ corresponds to /n/ only in Tancoco, in some specific items

One of the differences observed exclusively in Tancoco (#7) refers to the segment /l/, which in Tancoco is attested as /n/ in word-final contexts in some specific lexical items, but not in the rest of the communities, as distributed in Map 9.

Map 9 Distribution of /l/ and /n/ in word-final Contexts in Some Specific Lexical Items 

Table 2 Some examples that show the correspondence of /n/ in word-final context in Tancoco whereas the others have /l/ 

ITEM TCO CHIP SFC
‘it's on the edge’ k’ʷahat tin waːn k'ʷahat tu wal k'ʷahat ti b'aːʔ teːʔ
‘he eats’ hayun k'apuw hayul
‘it's in the middle’ k’ʷahat tam čehen kʷa tam č'ehel k'ʷahat tam č'ehel
‘rainbow’ pič'an pič'al pič'al
‘sea’ puhan puhal pulik ˈhaʔ

The origin of this particular change is not clear to me since it only affects the segment /l/ in word-final contexts in specific lexical items, but it is not extended as a regular change in the whole system. Nevertheless, it tends to occur in a regular way with both the persons consulted (male and female) of Tancoco, which shows that this is not a phonetic alternation. This is not an isolated feature that distinguishes Tancoco from San Francisco, as it will be shown later.

4.2 Vocalic Alternation of /a/ ~ /o/

One phonological change found in San Francisco Chontla (#8) that clearly contrasts with Chinampa de Gorostiza (#9) and Tancoco (#7) is the vocalic alternation of the vowel /a/ in some lexical items, which has changed to /o/ only in San Francisco but not in the other two (see Table 3 and Map 10)

Map 10 Vowel /a/ Is Performed as /o/ in Some Lexical Items Only in Chontla 

Table 3 Phonological /a/ Is Performed as /o/ in SFC, Contrasting with CHIP and TCO 

Lexical item TCO (7) CHIP (9) SFC (8)
‘walking stick’ kʷajap kʷajap k'ojop
‘he listened’ ʔač’aʔ ʔačʼaʔ ʔoč'oš
‘anyone’ šawaːkič hawaʔkič šowaːkič

This feature belongs to the phonological sphere, since it is not restricted to a particular context and because it is regular, given that the alternation is always between these two vowels. It is not an absolute rule that every /a/ will become an /o/ in San Francisco and in that sense this change is apparently linked to specific lexemes.

4.3 Some other vocalic alternations in Tancoco, San Francisco Chontla and Chinampa de Gorostiza

There is a group of lexical items that present diverse types of alternation in the syllabic nucleus and, in some cases, the loss of a syllable. Clearly, some of the vocalic alternations are phonetically motivated by being adjacent to a palatal consonant-for example-but this is not as systematic as it could be expected.

Even though there is not a rule that determines the occurrences of these vowel alternations, they should be mentioned since they belong to the diversification that is currently taking place in this group of communities that has traditionally been considered a single dialectal unit (see Table 4 for examples of it).

Table 4 Vowel Alternations between Communities that Belong to Eastern Huastec 

LEXICAL ITEM TCO CHIP SFC
‘female turkey’ potoč putuč k'aθaːw
‘like that’ ʔančanaːʔ ʔančeːʔ ʔinčanaːʔ
‘my bone’ nu beklek nu beklek nu beklak
‘nine’ beleːho beleːho beleːhu
‘this one’ ʔešeːʔ ʔašeːʔ ʔašeːʔ
‘eight’ wašek wašik wašik
‘ten’ laːho laːhu laːhu
‘warm’ mamomuːl mamumuːl mamušː
‘beautiful’ ʔalbeːl ʔalabeːl ʔalobeːl
‘today’ šeweːʔ šoweːʔ šuweːʔ
‘I know him’ nan ʔu ʔešloːʔ nanaːʔ u ʔešlaːl nanaːʔ u ʔešlaːl
‘boy’ čakam čakam čikam k w itoːl
‘honey’ čiːmal ʔi čapča čiːmal ʔi čapt̥om čiːmal ʔi čapt̥am
‘rope’ laːθo laːθu laːθu
‘poor’ č’ewentaːl č'ontaːl č'ontaːl

As can be seen in Tables 2, 3 and 4, the three communities studied here show some degree of variation in the phonological level. As mentioned before, Kaufman (1985) had already noticed that Chontla seems to have a different evolution compared to other Huastec communities. Kaufman focuses specifically on the use of the phoneme /b'/ in Chontla, which is the only community that carries on the reflection of the proto-Mayan */b/. The data presented here reinforce the idea that there is a greater diversification at this grammatical level as it is clear that although the variation seems negligible it cannot be ignored.

4.4 Lexical Variation in Eastern Huastec

At the lexical sphere, the data tend to support a greater diversification of Eastern Huastec (see Tables 5, 6 and 7 below).

Table 5 Distinct Lexical Items in the Eastern Huastec Communities Studied 

ITEM TCO CHIP SFC
‘who’ hišti hitaʔ tamaːʔ
‘what’ hišti ~ hont'o hont'o toneːy
‘which house’ hišti ʔin k'imaːʔ hawaʔ in k'imaːʔ huːn tana ʔataː
‘another’ ʔiš nahaːʔ ʔanuːʔ nuːwaʔ
‘nothing’ baːšataʔ baʔtaʔ ~ yaba hata b'a tantoː
‘pinole’7 pinoːl moʔoθ poːθ k’apneːl
‘maize tamale’ t’uːθ k'oyeːʔ kʷitoːm muluč
‘bed’ waytalaːb waytal čeyil
‘rich man’ riko koʔol ʔan meːlu8 meːluliθ
‘grandfather’ maːm9 ya taːtaʔ pulik paː
‘flat’ biliːy ʔoliθ~ʔoliliːh b'aθiθiːl

The items in Table 5 show a tendency to isolate the three communities; nevertheless, the data in Table 6 and 7 exhibit a grouping where, most of the times, San Francisco and Tancoco show clear differences, whereas Chinampa de Gorostiza groups with any of them (see below):

Table 6 Lexical diversification that tends to group CHIP with TCO, and differentiate them with respect to SFC 

ITEM TCO CHIP SFC
‘hat’ sombreːr sumbreːl~ somˈbreːl koʔalaːp
‘how’ hont'oːn ti hont'o ni toneːj
‘why’ halištiː halešti ni tiːʔ
‘parrot’ loːɾo loɾo kiʔaːm
‘water snail’ kaɾakol ~ ʔukul an haːʔ ʔukul šoːč
‘elbow’ totočel~tutučil tutuːč~koːdo tiyiːkʰ
‘arrow’ fleːča fleːča t̥it̥oklaːp
‘scissors’ tiheːɾas tiheɾaʰ kašuštalaːp
‘mattock’ ʔasadoːn ʔasadoːn čišimtalaːp
‘basket’ toknal toknal taknaːp
‘jícara (gourd bowl)’ kʷentoː ~kʷentu tuʔ~ kʷentu ušnap tuˀ
‘skirt’ ʔakištalaːp~ nawa ʔalkistal lakapʰ
‘mother’ naːna naːna miːm ~ maː
‘father’ papaː ~taːta taːta pajloːm ~ paːʰ
‘to bite’ čaʔuš čaʔuš k'atuš
‘to cut with an ax’ šilaːʔ šilaːʔ pokoʷ
‘food’ tolaːp tolaːp k'apneːl ~ teʔneːl
‘to swim’ kowal kowal laːt̥um
‘small’ čekat ~ čikat čikat čipiːl
‘ugly’ k'ak'aθ k'ak'aθ ʔatašič
‘to be seated’ bušuːl bušuːl k'ʷahat
‘wet’ ʔaːč' ʔaːč' čabaθ
‘thanks’ hikpat̥ hikpat̥ halbinčiːč
‘to pursue’ ʔaθinθaːl ʔaynaːl~ʔaθinθal pahnaːl

Table 7 Lexical diversification that tends to group CHIP with SFC and to differentiate them with respect to TCO 

ITEM TCO CHIP SFC
‘sin’ ʔataštalaːp walap walap
‘sandals’ waɾače pahap~ waɾaʧo pahap
‘zanate’ kʷerbo č'ok' č'ok'
‘nigua’10 tikiliːy ʔočel č'ak ʔočel č'ak
‘wooden spoon’ kučaradeteʔ kuʧaɾa ~weweθtuʔ weweθtuʔ
‘water container’ češiktu ~ mul wit'iːm wit'iːm
‘kinsman’ ʔebt̥aːl haʔuːp haʔuːp
‘to know’ ʔešlaːl~witaʔ čoʔoːp čoʔoːp
‘to hate’ čakun tilibnaːl tilibnaːl

After comparing the lexical data in Tables 5-7, San Francisco, clearly, has more Huastec words whereas Tancoco and Chinampa exhibit more Spanish loans. Although I have grouped Chinampa and Tancoco in Table 6, and Chinampa and San Francisco Chontla in Table 7, I do not want to imply that the loan matches are due to shared changes but simply to point out the variation that currently exists between them and the complexity of grouping in one or two dialects in light of the discrepancies found.

In this sense, the presence of Spanish loans in Tancoco and Chinampa is not necessarily anchored to the same historical moment, since the words for 'wooden spoon' or 'sandals' present synchronic alternation of the Huastec word and the Spanish loan in Chinampa but not in Tancoco, where it is only attested the Spanish loan. It means that loanword matches between Tancoco and Chinampa could have pass to Huastec in different historical moments and, consequently, are not shared changes. In terms of diachronic variation, this fact suggests that Spanish-Huastec contact has been more intense in Tancoco than in Chinampa, and less intense in San Francisco.

A possible explanation for the lexical matches shared by Chinampa and San Francisco Chontla could rest on geography: CHIP is located in the middle of the state freeway connecting Chontla-Naranjos. This has allowed the residents to keep commercial and cultural exchange,11 which would explain the lexical borrowings between these two communities.

On the other hand, Tancoco is geographically isolated from the other communities, which would explain the lack of similarities. The absence of current interaction among the communities would explain that most of the lexical matches between CHIP and TCO may be traced to pre-Hispanic or Colonial times.

The idea of independent recent evolution of Tancoco apart from Chinampa-and, seemingly, apart from Chontla, too-is supported by other grammatical traits beside the lexical data, which reveals Tancoco as the most innovative of the eastern Huastec group. Also, it supports the idea that San Francisco Chontla is the most conservative Huastec-speaker community, not only regarding to phonology but also in the lexical sphere.

4.5 Distribution of the different negation words in Eastern Huastec

A last issue regarding lexical variation is related to the distinct negative words registered: SFC, CHIP and TCO exhibit different negation morphemes. See examples 1-3:

1. ʔan čakam yabaʔ ø=neʔeč 12ti doctoːr

DET boy/little NEG ABS3=become PREP doctor

‘The boy will not become a doctor’ (CHIP)

2. ʔan čikam k w itoːl b’ap ø=neʔeč ti ʔilaːliš

DET little boy NEG ABS3=become PREP doctor

‘The boy will not become a doctor’ (SFC)

3. ʔan čakam ʔibaːč ø=neʔeč ti doctoːr

DET little/boy NEG ABS3=become PREP doctor

‘The boy will not become a doctor’ (TCO)

Map 11 Distribution of the different negation morphemes 

As shown in Map 11, each of the communities that belong to Sierra de Otontepec have different negation markers. On the contrary, the Central dialect (Tantoyuca, Veracruz) and those Huastec speaker communities located at San Luis Potosí (i.e. Western dialect) share the same negation mark, yab.

The negative lexemes attested in Huastec are very different to the lexical form reconstructed by Kaufman and Justeson (2003: 1531) in proto-Mayan *ma, and to the negative words attested in the rest of the Mayan languages. Huastec preserved the vowel /a/ and, possibly, the bilabial feature of the proto-Mayan negation marker. Considering that the prototypical Mayan root structure is CVC, proto-Huastec form could had been *b'ap, and the other lexical forms are derived from this.

4.6 Alternation of absolutive third person in Eastern Huastec

As a regular head-marking language, Huastec marks the different arguments of the verb through person morphemes in the verb. In most of the communities, the absolutive third person is expressed as a zero morpheme, except in SFC, where the consistent marking of 3rd person plural absolutive marker is Vb, as exemplified in 4-6:

4. babaːʔ ø=waʔč-i-n

3PL ABS3=to be born-inac-mpass

‘They were born’ (CHIP; Ver)

5. b'ab’aːʔ ʔub’=waʔč-i-n

3PL ABS3PL=to be born-INAC-MPASS

‘They were born’ (SFC; Ver)

6. hahaːʔt̥ik ø=waʔ-č -i-n

3PL ABS3=to be born-INAC-MPASS

‘They were born’ (TCO; Ver)

The proto-form was reconstructed as *ib by Meléndez (2011: 192), nevertheless the form could have been *ib' considering the Kaufman and Norman's (1984: 91) reconstruction in proto-Mayan as **eb', and also according to the phonological system presented by Norcliffe (2003) for proto-Huastec. The alternation of the vowel from /u/ to /i/ in the speaker community of San Francisco Chontla is explained as a regular alternation motivated by a process of vowel harmony that occurs with person markers (see Meléndez 2011 for a deeper discussion on this issue).

4.7 Variation of Syntactic Features in Eastern Huastec

There are different morphological features associated to the person system (see Meléndez 2011; Meléndez, 2017), as well as other relevant syntactic features.

a) It is a strict Huastec syntactic rule that the determiner position in a noun phrase must be always overt; i.e. no bare nominals are allowed as noun phrases. However, there is a weakening in the marking of the definite determiner in TCO (example 7) with respect to SFC and CHIP, whereas the last two keep the syntactic marking of the determiner in every context, as exemplified in 8:

7. ʔan pik’oʔ ø=k' w ahat ʔan ti wiːʔlep ø ʔataː

def dog ABS3=to be.cp def prep door house

'The dog was at the door of the house' (TCO; Ver)

8. ʔan pik’oʔ ø=k' w ahay ʔan t=in pikib ʔan ʔataː

def dog ABS3=to be.icp def prep=pos3 front def house

'The dog is at the door of the house' (SFC; Ver)

The weakening of the determiner occurs as well with the indefinite determiner, as in example 11 (in contrast to 9 and 10):

9. ø=koːʔol huːn ʔi laktem ʔan ʔu k'imaːʔ

ABS3=to.have one INDEF chair def pos1s home

'He has a chair at my home' (chip; Ver)

10. ø =waʔač huːn ʔi laktem ʔan ti ʔataːʔ

ABS3=there.is one INDEF chair DEF PREP house

'There is a chair at the house' (SFC; Ver)

11. waʔač huːn ø laktem ʔan ti ʔataːʔ

ABS3=there.is one chair DEF PREP house

'There is a chair at the house' (TCO; Ver)

b) Another syntactic feature that shows a divergence from Chinampa de Gorostiza compared to San Francisco and Tancoco is the syntactic structure of human-interrogative sentences. These kinds of sentences differ as follows: chip has [INT+ tin + FN] whereas TCO and SFC have [int+ fn]; examples 12-14 illustrate this feature:

12. ¿ hitaš tin t’ohliš ?

who ? major

‘who is the major?’ (chip)

13. ¿tama ʔan t’ohliɵ?

who def major

‘who is the major?’ (SFC)

14. ¿hišti ʔan aheːnte?

who def major

‘who is the major?’ (TCO)

The meaning and syntactic function of the morpheme tin in sentence 12 is not clear, since it is not attested in other Huastec variants, but in Chinampa it tends to occur frequently in interrogative sentences. However, it is interesting that this innovation occurred just in Chinampa, where the language is barely spoken by only a few elderly Huastec speakers.

Another innovation attested in Chinampa is the tendency to use complex verbal forms (nominalizations) whereas TCO and SFC tend to use simple verbal forms, see 15-17:

15. babaːʔ ʔešom ʔan ti t’ohnal

3PL PROGR DEF PREP work

‘They are working (now)’ (CHIP; Ver)

16. b'ab'aːʔ ʔešom t=ub’ t’ohnal

3PL PROGR DEF PREP=ABS3pl work

‘They are working (now)’ (SFC; Ver)

17. hahaːʔt̥ik ʔešom ti ø=t’ohnal

3PL PROGR DEF PREP ABS3=work

‘They are working (now)’ (TCO; Ver)

5. Central and Western Huastec: overlapping and Divergent isoglosses

The Central dialect is spoken in the communities located in the Tantoyuca municipality (Veracruz) and its surroundings, and it clearly exhibits particular grammatical features that diverge from the other dialects. As previous linguistic studies have pointed out, the major differences between Western and Central pertain to the phonological sphere and are related to the distribution of the phonemes /ȼ/ and /č/ in lexical items that clearly diverge from the other dialects (see Kaufman 1985; Ochoa 2007; Herrera Zendejas 2014).

The Western dialect is, geographically and numerically, the most widespread variant. It extends from the Xilitla and Aquismón Sierras (SLP) to the plains region adjacent to Veracruz. It is the only variant that has gained new spaces of use13 instead of losing them, a factor that strengthens the vitality of the language.

The matches between both dialects belong mainly to the morphological sphere. In the person system studied by Meléndez (2011, 2017) it is shown that they share one of the most important innovations of Huastec: they have reduced one structural position in the verbal phrase, marking only one of the two participants of transitive sentences. In the person system, they share as well the bi-morphemization of most of the morphemes that belong to this sphere (personal pronouns are bi-morphemic; the second and third person of ergative and absolutive sets are systematically bi-morphemic as well). The overlapping isoglosses are shown in Map 12.

Map 12 Overlapping morphological and syntactic isoglosses in Central and Western Huastec dialects 

The data show that the Central and Western dialects are phonologically differentiated. However, there are similarities both in their verbal structure and in morphological features related to person markers (the red and purple isoglosses in the Map 12 represent morphological similarities of this otherwise clearly different dialects). These similarities could be explained by three different causes: a) as a product of recent common evolution, b) by autonomous parallel evolution or c) by contact-induced recent innovations. Common evolution is not the most viable explanation, since the phonological divergences are the most ancient traits that produced the split into the current variants and these are not shared in both groups. Independent parallel evolution could be a possibility if the typological tendencies were common in other languages, but they are not. Contact-induced recent innovations could be the most optimal explanation, since the communities probably have maintained regular contact in the more recent history. This could explain the matches that exist in the lexical sphere and the clear differences with respect to the communities located in the Otontepec Region, in which Central Huastec speakers have clearly maintained themselves isolated. This particular topic needs to be explored in a specific work but it goes beyond the scope of the present study.

6. Discussion

Based on lexical and phonological data, Kaufman suggested that the differentiation between the Huastec dialects occurred in recent times. This fact is relevant for this research since it is clear that Huastec dialect diversification is not anchored to a remote past and that makes a significative difference with respect to other Mesoamerican languages, like the Otomanguean languages, for instance, where diversification is so antique that they have evolved into different languages. Although it is undeniable that the variants are well differentiated as discrete entities, they show a recent evolution that is still in progress, given that it is possible to observe synchronic alternations in some morphemes, phonemes and lexical items. In this sense, there are at least three dialectal variants of Huastec, but the evolution attested within Eastern Huastec deserves a deeper look into the evidence. Then, the question arises: how different two speaker communities need to be in order to be considered different dialects, or even different languages? The answer is not properly a linguistic task, but it can be useful to understand the current variation and its applications for practical or political matters. San Francisco, Chinampa and Tancoco have shown differences that must not be ignored; if they cannot be considered different dialects, the variation of the grammar levels between Tancoco and San Francisco Chontla should be enough to study them independently (at least linguistically).

The communities located in Sierra de Otontepec have more reflections of proto-Huastec than those located in Tantoyuca, Veracruz, and in the State of San Luis Potosí. The Sierra de Otontepec speakers, in towns like San Francisco, seem conservative compared with speakers from Tancoco and Chinampa, who are innovative, even when Huasteco speakers are in their sixties or older.

Another question arises: why are these variants changing even when the language is not being used? There is no obvious explanation for the fact these changes are happening in less linguistically-vital communities (Tancoco and Chinampa), as well as in communities where the language is widely spoken (San Luis Potosí).

The intra-dialectal morpho-syntactic differences in the Otontepec or Eastern Huastec are remarkable and they contribute to separate it into three different groups, considering that their internal differences are systematic in other grammatical levels (CHIP, TCO and Chontla). Finally, the morphosyntactic differences in the Western or slp dialect are subtler than in the Eastern dialect: this is interesting since their geographical distribution is very extensive compared with the Otontepec dialects; but, nevertheless, it seems to conserve many shared grammatical features between all the speaker communities that belong to this dialectal group.

Some theories try to explain this complex sociolinguistic variation based on the fact that Sierra de Otontepec remained isolated from the rest of the Huastec-speaking communities. Also, San Francisco Chontla is separated, both geographically and socially, from Tancoco as there is not a road or social system connecting their inhabitants.

It is not surprising that San Luis Potosí has so many dialectal innovations, as it is the most widespread Huastec dialect, with a high linguistic vitality. An important feature to underline is the recent morphological innovations shared with the Central dialect, which is clearly differentiated in phonological matters. These innovations must be recent, because they are not attested in Tapias Zenteno’s registry from 1767, and they some show synchronic alternation.

References

Caz Cho, Sergio. 2007. Informe de variación dialectal en Q'eqchi'. Antigua: Oxlajuuj Keej Maya' Ajtz'iib'. [ Links ]

Coseriu, Eugenio.1991 [1977]. La geografía lingüística. In: Coseriu, Eugenio (ed.), El hombre y su lenguaje (estudios de teoría y metodología lingüística), 2nd edition. Madrid: Gredos. [ Links ]

De la Cruz, Juan. 1571. Doctrina cristiana en la lengua Guasteca, con la lengua castellana. Mexico: Pedro Ocharte. [ Links ]

Embriz Osorio, Arnulfo & Oscar Zamora Alarcón. 2012. México: Lenguas indígenas nacionales en riesgo de desaparición. Mexico: Instituto Nacional de Lenguas Indígenas. [ Links ]

Espinosa Ruiz, Alma Rosa. 2015. La tradición cerámica huaxteca de la Sierra Gorda. Mexico: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México. (MA thesis). [ Links ]

García Mouton, Pilar. 1996. Dialectología y geografía lingüística. In: Alvar López, M. (ed.), Manual de dialectología Hispánica. El español de América, 63-77. Madrid: Ariel. [ Links ]

Herrera Zendejas, Esther. 2014. Mapa fónico de las lenguas mexicanas: formas sonoras (1 y 2). Mexico: El Colegio de México. [ Links ]

Instituto Nacional de Lenguas Indígenas. 2008. Catálogo de las lenguas indígenas nacionales: Variantes lingüísticas de México con sus autodenominaciones y referencias geoestadísticas. http://www.inali.gob.mx/clin-inali/ Consulted on June 13, 2017. [ Links ]

Kaufman, Terrence. 1985. Aspects of Huastec Dialectology and Historical Phonology. International Journal of American Linguistics, 51(4): 473-476. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1086/465937 [ Links ]

Kaufman, Terrence. n.d. Lista de elicitación para el diagnóstico dialectal de las lenguas mayas de Guatemala. (ms. in possession of the author). [ Links ]

Kaufman, Terrence & John Justeson. 2003. A Preliminary Mayan Etymological Dictionary. http://www.famsi.org/reports/01051/index.html (Consulted on November 9, 2017). [ Links ]

Kaufman, Terrency S. & William M. Norman. 1984. “An Outline of Proto-Cholan Phonology, Morphology and Vocabulary.” In: Phoneticism in Maya Hieroglyphic Writing. Albany: Institute of Mesoamerican Studies, State University of New York. pp.77-176. [ Links ]

Kondic, Snježana. 2012. A grammar of South Eastern Huastec, a mayan language from Mexico. Sydney-Lyon: University of Sydney, University of Lyon. [ Links ]

Meléndez, Lucero. 2011. El sistema de persona en protohuasteco. Mexico: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México. (PhD thesis). [ Links ]

Meléndez, Lucero. 2017. Algunas isoglosas morfológicas del huasteco o teenek: propuesta metodológica hacia la construcción de un diagnóstico dialectal. In: Guerrero, Alonso & Orozco Leonor. (eds.), Estudios de variación geolingüística, Mexico: Instituto Nacional de Antropología e Historia. pp. 485-514. [ Links ]

Norcliffe, Elizabeth. 2003. "The historical reconstruction of Proto-Huastecan", Christchurch: University of Canterbury (MA thesis). [ Links ]

Smith Stark, Thomas. n.d. Cédula para la elicitación preliminar de sintaxis y morfología (ms. in possession of the author). [ Links ]

Ochoa, Angela. 2007. El huasteco veracruzano y el huasteco potosino. Dos variantes de un mismo idioma. In: Ochoa, L. (coord.), Cinco Miradas en torno a la Huaxteca. Xalapa: Gobierno del Estado de Veracruz, Consejo Veracruzano de arte popular. 37-44. [ Links ]

Ochoa, Angela. 1984. El idioma huasteco de Xiloxúchil, Veracruz. Mexico: Instituto Nacional de Antropología e Historia . [ Links ]

Pfeiler, Barbara Blaha. 1995. “Variación fonológica en el maya yucateco.” In: Vitalidad e influencia de las lenguas indígenas en Latinoamérica. Eds. Ramón Arzápalo Marín and Yolanda Lastra. Mexico: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México. Instituto de Investigaciones Antropológicas, pp.488-497. [ Links ]

Pfeiler, Barbara Blaha. 1996. “Yaan difereensia waye’ yéetel máaya yukatáan (un estudio dialectal)”. Los mayas de Quintana Roo. Investigaciones antropológicas recientes, Arbeitsblätter, Nm. 14. Ed. Ueli Hostettler. Universität Bern, Institut für Etnologie, pp. 7-11. [ Links ]

Pfeiler, Barbara Blaha. 1997. “El xe’ek’ y la jach maya: Cambio y futuro del maya ante la modernidad cultural en Yucatán”. In: Convergencia e individualidad. Las lenguas mayas entre hispanización e indigenismo (Colección americana 7), pp. 125-140. Andreas Koerchert and Thomas Stolz (Eds.), Hannover: Verlag für Ethnologie. [ Links ]

Pfeiler, Barbara Blaha & Andrew Hofling. 2006. “Apuntes sobre la variación dialectal en el maya yucateco” Península Vol. I, Nm. 1, pp. 27-44. [ Links ]

Pisson Alcaraz, Perla. 2016. Análisis lingüístico de las recetas de cocina tének de El Mamey San Gabriel, Tantoyuca, Veracruz. Mexico: Escuela Nacional de Antropología e Historia. (BA Thesis). [ Links ]

Romero, Sergio. 2006. "Sociolinguistic Variation and Linguistic History in Mayan: the Case of K'iche'". Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania. (PhD thesis). [ Links ]

Tapia Zenteno, Carlos de. 1767. Paradigma apologético y noticia de la lengua huasteca, con vocabulario, catecismo y administración de sacramentos. Mexico: Bibliotheca Mexicana. [ Links ]

1This region is located in the Peninsula of Yucatán, Tabasco and Chiapas, Mexico, and extends to Guatemala, Belize and parts of El Salvador

2Henceforth, CLIN, by its Spanish acronym.

3Henceforth, INALI, by its Spanish acronym.

4The data and results presented here belong to the project "Documentación Lingüística del Huasteco," sponsored by INALI. This project pretends to contribute to a deeper understanding of Huastec variation. The research took three different fieldwork seasons and was later transcribed by a team formed by: Perla Pisson Alcaraz, Marysa Georgina Neri Velázquez, Nelly Iveth del Ángel Flores, Francisco Javier Ariano Cifuentes, Rodrigo Díaz Torres, Diego Mendoza Hernández and Carlos del Angel Mártir.

5The sociolinguistic approach is one of the less-studied fields in most Mayan languages and in Huastec as well. There are some recent works on Mayan dialectology that need to be mentioned here since they were consulted and partially guided this research: on Yucatec, Pfeiler (1995; 1997; 1996) and Pfeiler & Hofling (2006); on K'iche', Romero (2006); and on Q'eqchi', Caz (2007). However, the theoretical principles and the methodology followed in those studies differ from mine; that is why they are not being compared here.

6The phonetic symbols used in this paper are from the American Phonetic Alphabet, which has been commonly used to represent Mayan Languages. The equivalences in ipa are: <¢> = <ʦ>; <č> = <ʧ>; <b'> = <ɓ>; <t̥> = <ʈ>; <č̥> = <ʈʂ>; <š> = <ʃ>

7Toasted maize powder mixed with sugar and cinnamon. The word pino:l is a loanword from nahuatl pinolli which is attested only in Tancoco but not in the other two communities. In San Francisco, for instance, an expression that literally means 'powder meal' is used.

8meːlu is the Huastec word for 'money' and it is a loanword from Spanish <dinero>, 'money'. In chip 'rich man' is expressed as a sentence koʔol ʔan meːlu, [he] has def money, which literally means 'he has money' but that is used as the regular expression for translating this meaning. In San Francisco Chontla, speakers use more naturally an adjective formed from meluː-l-iθ 'dinero-?-der.adj which literally expresses 'wealthy.' Both items are clearly related but they partially express a different meaning.

9Most of Huastec kinship terms have been replaced by Spanish loans but this here is not the case since the three of them have Huastec roots to refer to 'grandfather.' However, the three communities compared show different expressions to verbalize the same meaning: TCO preserves the original reconstructed term for 'grandfather,' attested as well in the Colonial Huastec registers. In chip a nominal phrase is used to express, literally, 'our father;' the consulted persons in SFC used the nominal phrase that literally means 'big father.'

10This term refers to a kind of acarus that is very common in tropical areas.

11The person I consulted told me her family lives in a small community that can only be accessed by a dirt road starting at Chinampa.

12The verb neʔeč is normally used with the sense of 'to go' when it appears as nucleus of the verbal phrase. In many other contexts, it works as an auxiliary verb that codifies 'future.' In these examples, it is used as nucleus of the verbal phrase but with a meaning similar to 'to become.' Nevertheless, it is not frequent to find it with this last meaning in the other variants of Huastec, which could be part of a regional specific meaning. In any case, here, it works as an irregular verb and that is the reason it is not inflected by aspect markers.

13 http://nenek.inali.gob.mx/ES/; https://es-la.facebook.com/NenekMexico/; http://www.cdi.gob.mx/ecosgobmx/xeant.php. It is also noticeable that most of the linguistic descriptive work has been done in Potosino Huastec by native speakers as well as by non-native speakers.

Received: July 20, 2017; Accepted: January 19, 2018

Creative Commons License This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License