SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.4 número2El pensamiento creativo y su relación con el ciclo menstrual índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Revista

Articulo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • No hay artículos similaresSimilares en SciELO

Compartir


Journal of behavior, health & social issues (México)

versión impresa ISSN 2007-0780

J. behav. health soc. ISSUES vol.4 no.2 Cuernavaca nov. 2012

https://doi.org/10.5460/jbhsi.v4.2.34111 

Artículos empíricos

 

Sex differences in the visuospatial sketchpad in scholar children

 

Diferencias sexuales en la agenda viso-espacial de niños escolares

 

Miguel Ángel Hernández-Balderas, Gloria Rángel-Félix, Juan Carlos Zavala-González, Helena Romero-Romero, Juan Felipe Silva-Pereyra, Irma Yolanda del Rio-Portilla, Ma. de Lourdes Luviano-Vargas, Anaid Juanita Vera-Romero, Vicente Guerrero-Juárez, Jorge Bernal-Hernández

 

Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Facultad de Estudios Superiores Iztacala, Estado de México, México.

 

Autor de correspondencia

 

Received: August 15,2012
Revised: September 10, 2012
Accepted: Octubre 19, 2012

 

Abstract

This paper studies sex differences in working memory related to the visuospatial sketchpad and its interaction with the central executive through performance of a dual task with different levels of difficulty. Fourteen boys and 14 girls between 9 and 10 years old performed a memory task (primary task) with 4 levels of difficulty corresponding to 4 memory load levels, and a Go/No-go task as a visuospatial stimuli processing task (secondary task). The results demonstrated that the increase in the difficulty level in the primary task makes subjects have fewer correct responses in the secondary task; however, this only affected males significantly. These results might be a consequence of the amount of resources given by the central executive to perform the tasks: more resources were given to accomplish the primary task than to the secondary task, affecting the performance of the second. One may concluded that the relations between the central executive (processing) and the visuospatial sketchpad (storage) seem to be determined by a higher resource demand required by the storage in the memory, to the detriment of the processing activities of the central executive.

Key words: Sex differences in children, visuospatial sketchpad.

 

Resumen

En la presente investigación se estudiaron las diferencias sexuales en la memoria de trabajo relacionadas con la agenda viso-espacial y la interacción ejecutivo central-agenda viso-espacial mediante una tarea dual con diferentes niveles de dificultad. Participaron 14 niñas y 14 niños con edades entre 9 y 10 años, quienes realizaron una tarea de almacenamiento (tarea primaria), con 4 niveles de carga en la memoria y una tarea Go/No-Go como tarea de procesamiento de estímulos viso-espaciales (tarea secundaria). Los resultados mostraron que a mayor nivel de carga en la tarea primaria, los sujetos tuvieron significativamente menos aciertos en la tarea secundaria, sin embargo esto afectó significativamente solo a los niños. Lo anterior pudo deberse a que el ejecutivo central destinó mayores recursos a la tarea primaria que a la secundaria lo que afectó el desempeño en ésta última. Se puede concluir que las relaciones entre el ejecutivo central (procesamiento) y la agenda viso-espacial (almacenamiento) parecen estar determinadas por una mayor demanda de recursos que exige el almacenamiento en la memoria, en detrimento de las actividades de procesamiento del ejecutivo central.

Palabras clave: Diferencias sexuales en niños, agenda viso-espacial.

 

Introduction

Working memory (WM) is a system of limited capacity, simultaneously responsible for processing and temporarily storing information (Baddeley, 1986). According to Baddeley's model (2000), WM has four subsystems. The central executive (CE), which controls the flow of assigned attention resources to process or store (by reviewing) information (Cowan, 2005; Engle, Kane, & Tuholski, 1999). This is to say, it is an attention control system capable of focusing and changing attention resources, but it does not have storage capacity (Baddeley, 2003). The phonological loop (PL), which retains and manipulates verbal material, is composed of two components: the phonological store and the articulatory rehearsal component; the first is a memory store that is able to retain information based on speech for a short period of time (for example, read words), and the second is responsible for two different functions: changing visual information into a speech-based code, and placing it into the phonological store and renewing the information at the phonological store, counteracting the decay process. The visuospatial sketchpad (VSSP) processes and stores visual and spatial information. The episodic buffer processes both verbal and visuospatial information. Of these components, the CE and the PL are the most studied; not so the VSSP and the episodic buffer.

The VSSP works in the recognition and manipulation of objects, in academic tasks and in the memory of places, in mathematical and scientific thinking (Delgado, & Prieto, 2004), in the representation and manipulation of visual information, in solving of visuospatial problems, and takes an important role in disciplines of knowledge such as engineering, architecture, physics, chemistry and surgery (Sorby, & Baartmans, 1996). Aside from participating in spatial orientation activities, the VSSP is fundamental in reading comprehension and mental calculation (Jones, & Morris, 1992). According to Logie (1995) and Pickering, Gathercole, Hall, & Lloyd (2001) the VSSP is divided into two subcomponents: visual cache that storages color an visual forms, and an inner scribe related to spatial information and movement; this information is reviewed at the visual cache and then is transferred to the CE to be processed.

According to WM definition, its capacity must be by measured employing tasks that simultaneously impose demands in the processing and storage of information (Alloway, & Archibald, 2008); this would demand resources both for the CE (processing) and for the corresponding store (visual and/or visuospatial). The way in which resources are distributed allows evaluating the relations between the CE and each of the other components. For example, in a verbal dual task, the execution of the subjects is less efficient when the primary task is more complex or insofar as the secondary task also involves verbal stimuli (Hunt, & Ellis, 1999). This may be due to the high level of demand made to the CE, since attention resources are limited and must be distributed to the execution of two tasks as well as interference causing the use of the same type of information on both tasks. However, the use of simple tasks in the WM studies is frequent (Jenkin, Myerson, Hale, & Fry 1999). ;Factors that influence the capacity of the cognitive functions like the WM include the age and sex. In this field, the investigations are oriented to the study of the differences between men and women, establishing contrasts between both in relation to verbal and visuospatial abilities. Some studies (Coluccia, & Louse, 2004; Kimura, 1996; Levine, Vasilyeva, Lourenco, Newcobe, & Huttenlocher, 2005) show that women perform better at verbal tasks and verbal articulation, learn to write and read more quickly, and demonstrate a greater capacity in perceptual speed and visual memory (both functions related with the left side of the brain), while men have a better performance at visuospatial tasks like spatial visualization (ability in the use of analytical strategies to manipulate spatial information), spatial perception (body orientation in the space), mental rotation of two- or three-dimensional figures, measuring speed and precision, form recognition, left-right discrimination, representation of two-dimensional objects into three-dimensional objects and unfolding visual forms into complete sets (functions related with the right side of the brain) (Gil-Verona, Macias, Pastor, Paz, Barbosa, Maniega et al., 2003; Goldberg, 2001).

In a study where a meta-analyses was made, Torres, Gomez-Hil, Vidal, Puig, Bogey, & Sala-mero (2006) also showed that women perform at higher levels in verbal fluency, perceptual speed, memory and verbal learning, and men perform better at visuospatial ability, math problem and visual memory. Additionally, in a more recent analysis of the literature, there was reported superior execution in men in some visuospatial tasks, such as object localization (Andreano, & Cahill, 2009). Other authors had observed these effects in experimental studies (Lawton, & Hatcher, 2005; Singh, & Mishra, 2004). In a study that evaluated age and sex in youth and the elderly during passive spatial tasks (discrimination and perceptual memory of the distance) and in active tasks such as mental rotation, higher performance was observed in men during the mental rotation task, while women performed more highly during the other tasks (lachini, Ruggiero, Ruotolo, & Pizza, 2008). However, Harness, Jacot, Scherf, White, & Warnick (2008) showed than women perform better than men in verbal as well as visuospatial tasks.

In contrast to the previous studies, Feingold (1988) found that sex-based differences in verbal fluency and mathematical problem-solving are moderated or absent in some cases, while in visuospatial processing tasks the differences are very conspicuous, on account of which it may be supposed that the WM, and particularly the VSSP, is involved in these differences. Nonetheless, some studies deny the existence of sex-based differences in cognitive abilities. Hyde (2005) maintains that men and women are more cognitively similar than is reflected in studies on sex-based differences. Moreover Rahman, Bakare, & Serinsu (2011) show that these differences do not exist while Torres, Gomez-Gil, Vidal, Puig, & Salamero (2006) did not find differences in tasks that measure attention. In studies involving adolescents, there were no sex differences regarding recall of verbal stimuli (words) and image recall (lonescu, 2008).

In the few studies that have directly approached WM related to sex, we found equally contradictory results. Iachini, Ruggeiro, Ruo-tolo, & Pizza (2008) found superior execution in women, relative to men, in the visuospatial WM measured through the Corsi's block task. Other studies have reported a better performance for men at WM tasks of arithmetic type (Lynn, & Irwing, 2008) and authors such as Torres, Gómez-Gil, Vidal, Puig Boget, & Salamero (2006) did not find differences at the WM tasks. So, the lack of consistency is evident in studies about the sex differences in the various cognitive aspects that have been studied, including the WM. However, the results seem to show more evidence of differences between the subjects of different sex even if there is clarity missing.

Another possibility to understand the sex differences in visuospatial processing could be the study of results provided by investigations made in subjects of different ages, especially at early ages, since at these stages the role of experience could be less important than the natural biological mechanisms of every male or female subject.

Authors like Clements-Stephens, Rimrodt, & Cutting (2009) argue that studying the differences based on sex during the visuospatial processing in children and teens could provide evidence about the maturation of important regions involved in this type of processing, and also identify the development of the sex differences due to the use of strategies, which are in turn influenced by the experience. However, investigations conducted with children of different ages are very few so far.

Several investigations have shown that differences between men and women arise around preschool age or during the first primary school year. Levine, Huttenlocher, Taylor, & Langrock (1999) found that, on average, preschool-aged children are more precise than girls at tasks that measure precision in spatial transformations. They concluded that sex differences are present in spatial tasks since the 4 and half years old. However, Tzuriel, & Egozi (2010) suggest that the sex differences observed in children could be the consequence of other factors, such as training, since when men and women are trained in mental rotation tasks, there is no any difference observed between them.

As it happens in the adult case, the few studies that directly research the WM are not consistent either. Sánchez, Taballo, Marro, Sánchez, Yorio, & Segura (2009) carried out a study about de WM develop in children. They found that the performance improves with age, as in the case of recall and recognition of visuospatial patterns, although it is not clear if this improvement is the product of an increase in sketchpad capacity during infancy or changes in strategy that affect the amount of information that can be retained. In this study it was also observed that sex differences are present since childhood, because boys have a better performance than girls at visuospatial tasks. However, in a similar study, boys performed worse than girls; this result was interpreted as a cerebral immaturity in boys related to girls, between 6 and 10 years old (Vuaontela, Steenari, Carlson, Kolvisto, Fjälberg, & Aronen, 2003).

Some authors highlight that these differences could be caused by the spatial processes evaluated, among which the WM is emphasized (Iachini, Ruggiero, Ruotolo, & Pizza, 2008). Inside the so-called "spatial memory," one may distinguish to visuospatial WM, memory for object location, memory for routes and sequential spatial information (Kessels, De Haan, Kappelle, & Postma, 2001; Kesses, Postma, Kappelle, & De Haan, 2002; Postma, 2000; Postma, Jager, Kessels, Hans, Koppeschaar, & Van Hok, 2004). However, even in studies where the WM is mentioned as the principal object of study, its role is rarely well-specified. Additionally, there are no pure measures of this type of processing, because some images can be coded either semantically or phonologically (Pickering, 2001). For example, Palmer (2000) reported that children under 8 years codify the images visually, but after that age they tend to use phonological codification for its recall (see also Hitch, Halliday, Schaafstal, & Scharaagen, 1988). Some authors like Robert, & Savoie (2006) suggest that the lack of consensus in the studies about sex differences might be caused by the fact that many tasks that make either verbal or visuospatial demands in WM tasks only make storage demands explicit. These authors provide evidence that, in comparison with simple tasks, dual tasks are better at showing sex differences in the verbal and visuospatial WM (see also Cronoldi, & Vecchi, 2003) and conclude that women perform better than men when the task requires, besides storage, either a verbal or visuospatial process (Kaufman, 2007). In this context it has been observed that the sex differences in spatial orientation only emerge when the tasks require a high load of the visuospatial WM (Coluccia, & Louse, 2004). Thus, it seems that studies on the visuospatial WM in which dual tasks are used and in which high demands are made to memory load are those which present more consistent results in relation with sex differences in spatial ability.

The purpose of this paper was to compare the visuospatial WM in boys and girls of school age, using a dual task with different load in the VSSP store, aside from establishing how the CE and visuospatial interact in this type of tasks.

 

Method

Subjects

Fourteen girls ( = 9.7 years) and 14 boys ( = 9.6 years) participated, all of whom attend public schools in Mexico City. They were in the academic school grade corresponding to their age. All children voluntarily participated after an announcement was made in 2 schools. Children were selected with an intellectual coefficient > 85 according to the WISC-R. They had a normal neurological and neuropsychological evaluation that was made by a neuropsychologist and a neurologist.

Task

The children performed a dual task with simultaneously demand of processing and storing visuospatial stimulus and 4 levels of memory load. At the beginning of each session, the instructions were shown on the computer screen. The experimental phase consisted of the execution of a primary and a secondary task (figure 1). At the primary task there were presented 3x4 cells matrices with a number of cells randomly filled according to the memory load level (figure 2) which were needed to be remembered by the subjects, while the secondary task consisted into a Go/No-Go visual task. The stimuli of which consisted this secondary task were arrows pointing to different directions. During the execution of the secondary task, subjects were instructed to "review in memory" the position of the filled cells of the given matrix in the primary task.

In the zero memory load (difficulty level 1), subjects only executed the secondary task, which consisted in a pseudo-random presentation of series of arrows in different direction (). The subjects were instructed to respond, as quick as possible, to the presentation of the target stimuli () by clicking the left button of the mouse, while the other stimuli did not require any type of response. The duration of each stimulus was 100 ms and had an inter-stimuli interval of 1.8-2.2 seconds. A total of 360 stimuli were presented divided into 4 blocks, 18 target stimuli and 72 frequent stimuli per block with a duration of 3 minutes and 4 seconds.

In the next 3 levels (i.e., low memory load, medium memory load and high memory load) the subjects performed both tasks at the same time. In the primary task, each block begins with the presentation of a matrix during 10 seconds for storage in the memory. Immediately afterward began the secondary task, which consisted in the Go/No-Go task (in the same manner that they did in the zero load condition). After this task was done, the recall stage began, where three different matrices were randomly presented one at a time where the one that was presented in the primary task was or was not included. The subject had to respond by clicking the left button of the mouse when the matrix was identified and clicking the right button when the matrix was not included. The matrix presented at the start of each block was different in each of them. Thus, each level included the following order of phases: the storage demands (matrix), processing (execute the Go/No-Go task) and recall. In these 3 levels, the duration of each block was 3 minutes, 25 seconds. The presentation of the matrices and the arrows was made in white with a black background on the center of the computer screen; the task has a total running time of 50 minutes, and was delivered by the STIM program (NeuroScan, 1995) on a computer.

Data analyses

To compare the performance of both sexes, and observe the differences between the memory load levels (level 1, 2, 3 and 4), the percentage of correct responses and reaction time on the secondary task, as well as the percentage of matrices-recalled in the primary task, were analyzed. At the secondary task, the percentage of correct responses and the average of the reaction times for each trial block were obtained, and the blocks for each load level were averaged. For the primary task, matrix-recalled was measured by obtaining the percentage of correctly-remembered matrices in each level (because each level consisted of 4 blocks and in each block a new matrix to remember was presented, a total of 4 matrices was presented per level). Before statistical analysis, the percentages were transformed using ARCSIN [SQRT (percentage/100)] to reach an approximation to the normal distribution. A series of ANOVAs was made with two factors: Sex (girls and boys) as an inter-subject factor and Memory load level (1, 2, 3 and 4) as an intra-subject factor, for the percentage of correct recall and percentage of correct responses as well as for reaction times. When there were two or more degrees of freedom at the numerator, the Greenhouse-Geisser correction was applied. Post hoc multiple tests were carried out by the Tukey test when necessary.

 

Results

Secondary task

As it is shown on Table 1, the main effect of the Memory load level was statically significant. On Figure 3 and table 2, it is shown that as the level in the Memory load increased, i.e. as the matrices had a larger number of filled cells, the subjects had fewer correct responses in the secondary task, although in the level 4, with higher load, the number correct responses slightly increased. However, the post hoc analyses only showed significant differences between the percentages obtained at the zero memory load versus low memory load (p < .001), versus medium memory load (p < .001) and versus high memory load (p < .001). Thus since the level in which the visuospatial load started to increase, the performance at the secondary tasks significantly decreased.

Although the main effect of the Sex was not statically significant, there was a significant Sex by Memory load level interaction (see tables 1 and 2 and figure 4). In the low memory load level, boys and girls had similar performance, but when the memory load increased, in the medium memory load level for example, the number of correct responses decreased in both groups, but in the girls the decrease was moderate while in the boys, it was more severe. In accordance to the post hoc analyses, these sex differences were significant in the low memory load (p = .01) and in the medium memory load (p = .004). Likewise, the between group post hoc comparisons (boys versus girls) showed that in the boys the number of correct responses significantly decreased as the Memory load level increased (table 2).This was observed in the comparisons of the zero memory load level versus low memory load level (p < .001), versus medium memory load level (p<.001) and versus high memory load level (p<.001). On the other hand, in the girls, the differences in the percentage of correct responses related with the Memory load level were not significant.

The analysis of the reaction times related to the Memory load level, in the secondary task, did not show significant differences between groups (table 3). However, there was a marginal difference in the main effect of the Memory load level. As Table 4 shows, in boys and girls there was a tendency of increasing the reaction times as the Memory load level decreased.

Primary Task

As table 5 shows, in the primary task (memorization of matrices), there were no significant main effects or interactions. On table 6 one may notice that both groups remembered between 80% and 90% of the presented matrices, independently of the memory load level.

 

Discussion

Although the principal goal of this paper was the study of sex differences in the visuospatial processing, we will discuss the general results and then bring these findings into context with sex-related differences.

Processing and storing of information in the VSSP need attentional resources (supplied by the CE). These resources are limited but flexible, since they can be used in active way (categorize, take decisions, etc.) or in a passive way like it happens in the PL. This paper showed that the subjects maintained a uniform recall of the matrices independently of the memory load in the VSSP. However, memory load significantly affected the performance in the secondary task, showing that the CE was seriously affected in relation to the matrix complexity. This could be caused by the CE allocated more resources to the primary task and the resources designated to the secondary task were insufficient, and then the performance in the secondary task was severally affected. This effect may have been aggravated by the fact that in the primary and secondary tasks, the stimuli were visuospatial (Hunt, & Ellis 1999). Thus, we can observe that the relations between the CE (processing) and the VSSP (storage) seem to be determined by a huge demand of resources that require the storage in the memory, having in consequence a deficient CE.

In relation to the sex, in both groups were observed a similar performance in the recall and storage stages. However, it is clear that in the secondary task the boys were the most affected (Sex x Memory load interaction). This indicates that the boys had less correct responses in the secondary task as the memory load increased. Vuontela et al. (2003) and Levine et al. (1999) also observed that boys performed worse than girls in WM spatial tasks, but these authors do not specify how the different components of the WM interact. In accordance with our interpretation, the boys allocated less resources to the processing stage (secondary task) and more to the storage phase, while the girls allocated a sufficient amount of resources to have a similar performance as the boys. This allowed the girls rely on more resources than the boys in the processing stage having better performance than them (probably in terms of better strategy). Perhaps these different strategies are what Logie, & Pearson (1997) and Tzuriel, & Egozi (2010) refer to the differences in the visuospatial ability may depend on differences in the processing strategies affecting the amount of information that can be retained and processed simultaneously.

In the literature there are others factors mentioned that could influence the presence of sex differences such as the transformation of visuospatial information into phonological codes in infants older than 8 years (Palmer, 2000; Pickering, 2001). It is possible that the girls of this sample transformed the visual items into phonological codes (for example, finding similarities in the configuration of the matrix with some letter or figure).

Other possibility could be that independently of strategies, the demands for codification storage require more attentional resources from boys than from girls, which could impede the correct information processing. This could result in more errors in the secondary task for the boys. These differences may be observed only after an increase in the memory load. Thus, it seems that sex differences can only be observed from the tasks that test high WM storage demands. This agrees with the ideas expressed by Coluccia, & Louse (2004), who assure that the differences in spatial orientation only arise when the tasks require a high memory load of the visuospatial processing.

Based on the results of this study, we can affirm that the lack of uniformity in the difficulty level of the tasks used in previous investigations could be a factor that explains why in some studies there have not been observed sex-based differences either in the children or adults.

These results also show that the dual task used in this study was useful in differentiating between individuals of different sex. The fact that it involved a dual task where VSSP load was varied, gathered the necessary conditions to test this WM system, confirming the statements of Robert, & Savoie (2006), Cornoldi, & Vecchi (2003) and Kaufman (2007). However, it is important to mention that the task could not discriminate between the memory load differences, so it is important to pay attention to this factor.

The studies about visuospatial abilities, frequently found better performance in men, which shows the possible influence of the gonadal hormones in their performances. Some authors have proposed that these differences are related to the hemispheric specialization to which gonadal hormones contribute since birth (Gil Verona et al., 2003).

Contrary to these observations, in the present study, the girls performed better than the boys, what make us suppose that it is not until the adolescence that sexual difference begins to be expressed until it reaches its adult form, when hormonal development has finished. So, the hormonal factor could interact with the experience to solve paradigms of dual type that were used to study the WM.

Data on this paper showed higher performance for girls in visuospatial WM. Robert, & Savoi (2006) and Kaufman (2007) observed a similar trend between adult men and women, using a very similar task as this study. However, it is necessary to emphasize that this study used a small sample, which limits the generalization of the findings.

 

Conclusions

In this paper, it is clear that girls showed a better performance than boys in their processing abilities, in situations where the amount of load of visuospatial storage was increased. The memory load level in the tasks that measures the visuospatial WM capacity is an important factor in differentiating the performance between girls and boys in the visuospatial ability.

 

References

Alloway, T. P., & Archibald, L. (2008). Working memory and learning in children with developmental coordination disorder and specific language impairment. Journal of Learning Disabilites, 41, 251-262, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022219408315815        [ Links ]

Andreano, M. J., & Cahill, L. (2009). Sex influences on the neurobiology of learning and memory. Learning and Memory, 16, 248-266, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.918309        [ Links ]

Baddeley, A. D. (2003). Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Neuroscience, Nature Reviews, 4, 829-839, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1201        [ Links ]

Baddeley, A. D. (2000) The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, (11), 417-423, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2        [ Links ]

Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. New York: Oxford University Press.         [ Links ]

Clements-Stephens, A. M., Rimrodt, S. L., & Cutting, L. E. (2009). Developmental sex differences in basic visuospatial processing: Differences in strategic use? Neuroscience letters, 449, 155-160, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.10.094        [ Links ]

Coluccia, E., & Louse, G. (2004). Gender differences in spatial orientation: A review. Journal of enviromental psychology, 24, 329-340, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.08.006        [ Links ]

Cornoldi, C., & Vecchi, T. (2003). Visuo-spatial working memory and individual differences. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.         [ Links ]

Cowan, N. (2005).Understand ingintelligence: Asummary and an adjustable attention hypothesis. In O. Wilhelm, & R. W. Engle (Eds.), Understanding and measuring intelligence. (pp.469-488). New York: Sage        [ Links ]

Delgado, A. R., & Prieto, G. (2004) Cognitive mediators and sex related differences in mathematics. Intelligence, 32, 25-32, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(03)00061-8        [ Links ]

Engle, R. W., Kane, M. J., & Tuholski, S.W. (1999). Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence and functions of the prefrontal cortex. In A. Miyake y P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. (pp.102-134). New York: Cambridge University Press.         [ Links ]

Feingold, A. (1988) Cognitive gender differences are disappearing. American Psychologist, 43, 95-103, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.43.2.95        [ Links ]

Gil-Verona, J. A., Macías, J. A., Pastor, J. F., Paz, F., Barbosa, M., Maniega, M. A., Román, J. M., López, A., Alvarez-Alfageme, I., Rami-González, L., & Boget, T. (2003). Diferencias sexuales en sistema nervioso humano. Una revisión desde el punto de vista psiconeurobiológico. Revista Internacional de Psicología Clínica y de la Salud, 3 (2), 351-361.         [ Links ]

Goldberg, E. (2001) The Executive Brain. Frontal lobes and the civilized mind. OXFORD. University Press.         [ Links ]

Harness, A., Jacot, L., Scherf, S., White, A., & Warnick, E. J. (2008). Sex differences in working memory. Psychological reports, 103, 214-218.         [ Links ]

Hitch, G. J., Halliday, M. S., Schaafstal, A. M., & Scharaagen, J. M. C. (1998) Visual working memory in children. Memory and cognition, 16, 120-132.         [ Links ]

Hunt, R. R., & Ellis, H. C. (1999). Fundamentals of cognitive psychology (6th Ed.). Boston:McGraw-Hill.         [ Links ]

Hyde, J. S. (2005). The Gender similarities hyphotesis. American Psychologist, 60 (6), 581-592.         [ Links ]

Iachini, T., Ruggiero, G., Ruotolo, F., & Pizza, R, (2008). Cap 16. Age and gender differences in some components of spatial cognition. In H. T. Benninghouse et al. (Eds.), Women And Aging: New Research. Nova Science Publishers. Inc.         [ Links ]

Ionescu, M. D. (2008). Sex differences in memory and estimates for pictures and words. Psychological Reports, 87, 315-322.         [ Links ]

Jenkins, L., Myerson, J., Hale, S., & Fry, F. A. (1999). Individual and developmental differences in working memory across the life span. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6 (1), 28-40, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03210810        [ Links ]

Jones, D., & Morris, N. (1992). Irrelevant speech and serial recall: implications for theories of attention and working memory. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 33, 212-229, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.1992.tb00911.x        [ Links ]

Kaufman, S. B. (2007). Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial visualization ability: Can they be accounted for by differences in working memory capacity? Intelligence, 35, 211-223, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.07.009        [ Links ]

Kessels, R. P. C., De Haan, E. H. F., Kappelle, L. J., & Postma, A. (2001). Varieties of human spatial memory: A meta-analysis on the effects of hippocampal lesions. Brain Research Review, 35, 295-303, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0173(01)00058-3        [ Links ]

Kessels, R. P. C., Postma, A., Kappelle, L. J., & De Haan, E. H. F. (2002). Selective impairments in object-location binding, metric encoding and their integration after ischaemic stroke. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 115-129.         [ Links ]

Kimura, D. (1996). Sex, sexual orientation and sex hormones influence human cognitive function. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 6, 259-263, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(96)80081-X        [ Links ]

Lawton, A. C., & Hatcher, D. W. (2005). Gender differences in integration of images in visuospatial memory. Sex Roles, 53, (10), available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-7736-1        [ Links ]

Levine, S. C., Huttenlocher, J., Taylor, A., & Langrock, A. (1999). Early sex differences in spatial skill. Developmental Psychology, 35, 940-949, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.4.940        [ Links ]

Levine, S. C., Vasilyeva, M., Lourenco, F., Newcombe, N., & Huttenlocher, J. (2005). Socioeconomic status modifies the sex in spatial skill. Psychological Science, 16, 841-845, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01623.x        [ Links ]

Lynn, R., & Irwing, P. (2008). Sex differences in mental arithmetic, digit span, and g defined as working memory capacity. Intelligence, 37, 226-235, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2007.06.002        [ Links ]

Logie, R. (1995), Visuo-spatial working memory, Essays in Cognitive Psychology. pp. 126-131, Hove (UK): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.         [ Links ]

Logie, R. H., & Pearson, D. G. (1997). The inner eye and the inner scribe of visuo-spatial working memory: Evidence from developmental fractionation. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 241-257.         [ Links ]

NeuroScan (1995). STIM (Version 2.0). El paso, Texas, USA: NeuroScan Technical Support, Inc.         [ Links ]

Palmer, S. (2000). Working memory: A developmental study of phonological recoding. Memory, 8, 179-193, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/096582100387597        [ Links ]

Pickering, S. J. (2001). The development of visuo-spatial working memory, Memory, 9, 423-432, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658210143000182        [ Links ]

Pickering, S. J., Gathercole, S.E., Hall, M., & Lloyd, S. A. (2001). Development of memory for pattern and path: Further evidence for the fractionation of visuo-spatial memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 397-420.         [ Links ]

Postma, A. (2000). Taxonomy of spatial memory processes. Cognitive Processing. (ICSC 2000), 52.

Postma, A., Jager, G., Kessels, R. P. C., Koppeschaar, P. F. H., & van Honk, J. (2004). Sex differences for selective forms of spatial memory. Brain and Cognition, 54, 24-34, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00238-0        [ Links ]

Rahman, Q., Bakare, M., & Serinsu, C. (2011). No sex differences in spatial location memory for abstract designs. Brain Cognition, 76, (1), 15-19, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.03.012        [ Links ]

Robert, M., & Savoie, N. (2006). Are there gender differences in verbal and visuospatial working memory resources? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18 (3), 378-397, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09541440500234104        [ Links ]

Sánchez, F. J., Tabullo, A. J., Marro, C., Sánchez, M. L., Yorio, A. A., & Segura, E. (2009). Efectos del desarrollo en la memoria de trabajo y el aprendizaje de categorías en niños. Anuario de Investigaciones, 16, 307-312.         [ Links ]

Singh, B., & Mishra, S. (2004). Effects of gender and type of encoding on retention traits. Brain and Cognition, 54, 24-34.         [ Links ]

Sorby, S. A., & Baartmans, B. J. (1996). The developmental and assessment of a course for enhancing the 3-D spatial visualization skills of first year engineering students. Journal of Engineering Education, 83, 3, 301-307.         [ Links ]

Statsoft. (1984). Statistica (v. 4.5) [statistical package]. U. S. A.         [ Links ]

Torres, A., Gómez-Gil, E., Vidal, A., Puig, O., Boget, T., & Salamero, M. (2006). Gender differences in cognitive functions and influence of sex hormones. Actas Españolas Psiquiátricas, 34, 6, 408-415.         [ Links ]

Tzuriel, D., & Egozi, G. (2010). Gender differences in spatial ability of young children: The effects of training and processing strategies. Child development, 81, 5, 1417-1430, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.11117j.1467-8624.2010.01482.x        [ Links ]

Vuontela, V., Steenari, M., Carlson, S., Kolvisto, J., Fjálberg, M., & Aronen, E. (2003). Audiospatial and Visuospatial Working Memory in 6-13 Year Old School Children. Learning & Memory, 10, 74-81, available via: http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.53503        [ Links ]

 

Notes

This study was supported by Project PAPCA 2010-2011 N. 55, FES Iztacala, UNAM. Special thanks to Dulce Castillo for the translation assistance, to the anonymous referees for their helpful comments on the original manuscript and to the children and parents who gave their time for this research. Each author's contribution was as follows: MAHB: children evaluation, statistical analysis and article writing; GRF: children evaluation and article writing; JCZG: children evaluation and article writing; HRR: children evaluation and article revision; JSP: statistical analysis and article revision; IYRP: methodology assistance and article revision; LLV: children evaluation; AJVR: children evaluation, statistical analysis, and article translation; VG: neurological assessment; JBH: article writing, methodology participation , statistical analysis and general revision.

Self-reference for authors: 0
Self-reference for JBHSI: 0

 

Correspondencia:
Jorge Bernal Hernández PhD.
Laboratorio de Neurometría, FES Iztacala,
UNAM. Av. de los Barrios No. 1, Los Reyes Iztacala, Tlalnepantla,
Estado de México, C.P. 54090, México.
E-mail: jbernal@unam.mx

 

Information about authors:

Nombre: Miguel Ángel Hernández Balderas. Grado: Licenciado en Psicología. Adscripción: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, UNAM, Facultad de Estudios Superiores Iztacala, Laboratorio de Neurometría. Sistema de Universidad Abierta y Educación a Distancia, profesor de asignatura A. Línea de investigación: Estudio de los trastornos del aprendizaje infantil. Dirección Postal: Laboratorio de Neurometría. Edificio UIICSE, FES Iztacala, UNAM Ave. de los Barrios No. 1, Col. Los Reyes Iztacala, Tlalnepantla, Estado de México, C.P. 54090, México. Correo: mahbalderas24@yahoo.com.mx

Nombre: Gloria Rangel Felix. Grado: Licenciada en psicología. Adscripción: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Facultad de Estudios Superiores Iztacala. Laboratorio de Neurometría. Línea de investigación: Trastornos del aprendizaje infantil. Dirección postal: Laboratorio de Neurometría. Edificio UIICSE, FES Iztacala, UNAM, Ave. de los Barrios No. 1, Colonia Los Reyes Iztacala, Tlalnepantla, Estado de México, C.P. 54090, México. Correo: Gloriarf@live.com.mx

Nombre: Juan Carlos Zavala González. Grado: Licenciado en Psicología (Pasante). Adscripción: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México. Facultad de Estudios Superiores Iztacala. Laboratorio de Neurometría. Línea de investigación: Trastornos del aprendizaje infantil. Dirección postal: Laboratorio de Neurometría. Edificio UIICSE, FES Iztacala, UNAM, Ave. de los Barrios No. 1, Colonia Los Reyes Iztacala, Tlalnepantla, Estado de México, C.P. 54090, México. Correo: psic.zavala@gmail.com

Nombre: Helena Romero Romero. Grado: Doctor en Psicología. Adscripción: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México. Sistema de Universidad Abierta y Educación a Distancia. Profesor de Asignatura A y Ayudante de Profesor. Línea de investigación: Trabajo con Potenciales Relacionados con Eventos en niños con problemas de aprendizaje y déficit de atención. Dirección postal: Laboratorio de Neurometría. Edificio UIICSE, FES Iztacala, UNAM, Ave. de los Barrios No. 1, Colonia Los Reyes Iztacala, Tlalnepantla, Estado de México, C.P. 54090, México. Correo: helena.rxr@gmail.com

Nombre: Juan Felipe Silva Pereyra. Grado: Doctor en Ciencias. Adscripción: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Facultad de Estudios Superiores (FES) Iztacala, Proyecto de Neurociencias, Laboratorio de Neurometría. Línea de investigación: Psicofisiología de los procesos fonológicos, semánticos y sintácticos. Dirección postal: Unidad de Investigación Interdisciplinaria en Ciencias de la Salud y Educación (UIICSE), FES Iztacala, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Ave de los Barrios No. 1, Los Reyes Iztacala, Tlalnepantla Estado de México, México CP 54090. Correo: jsilvapereyra@gmail.com. Página web: www.jsilvapereyra.wordpress.com

Nombre: Irma Yolanda del Río Portilla. Grado: Doctorado, Facultad de Psicología, UNAM Adscripción: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Facultad de Psicología, Laboratorio de Sueño y Coordinación de Psicofisiología, Profr. Titular A. T.C. def. Línea de Investigación: Asimetría cerebral: lateralidad y diferencias sexuales. Función cerebral durante el sueño. Distinciones: Sistema Nacional de Investigadores I. Dirección postal: Facultad de Psicología, Laboratorio de Sueño; Edificio D, piso 2, Av. Universidad 3004, Col. Copilco-Universidad, México, D.F. C.P. 04510. Correo: iyrp@unam.mx

Nombre: Ma. de Lourdes Luviano Vargas. Grados: Maestría en Psicología con Residencia en Neuropsicología Clínica. Adscripción: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Investigación y Posgrado. FES-Iztacala. Líneas de investigación: Trastornos del Aprendizaje de la lectura en niños escolares. Dirección postal: Laboratorio de Neurometría. Edificio UIICSE, FES Iztacala, UNAM, Ave. de los Barrios No. 1, Col. Los Reyes Iztacala, Tlalnepantla, Estado de México, C.P. 54090, México. Correo: lluviano@campus.iztacala.unam.mx

Nombre: Anaid Juanita Vera Romero. Grado: Alumna Licenciatura en Psicología. Adscripción: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México. Facultad de Psicología. Línea de investigación: Trastornos del Aprendizaje de la Lectura y del déficit de atención. Dirección postal: Laboratorio de Neurometría. Edificio UIICSE, FES Iztacala, UNAM, Ave. de los Barrios No. 1, Colonia Los Reyes Iztacala, Tlalnepantla, Estado de México, C.P. 54090, México. Correo: anivr93@gmail.com

Nombre: Vicente Guerrero Juárez. Grado: Especialidad en Neurología en el Instituto Nacional de Neurología y Neurocirugía (Dr. Manuel Velasco Suárez). Adscripción: Instituto Nacional de Neurología y Neurocirugía (Dr. Manuel Velasco Suárez) adscrito al departamento de Urgencias y Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Facultad de Estudios Superiores Iztacala, adscrito a la carrera de medicina. Líneas de Investigación: Trastornos del desarrollo. Dirección Postal: Av. De los Barrios No. 1, Los Reyes Iztacala, Tlalnepantla, Edo. Méx. C.P. 54090, Tel. +52-555-6231333 ext 39726 ó 39796. Correo: vic.gue.j@gmail.com

Nombre: Jorge Bernal Hernández. Grado: Doctor en ciencias Fisiológicas. Adscripción: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, UNAM, Facultad de Estudios Superiores Iztacala, Laboratorio de Neurometría. Profesor Titular "C" Línea de Investigación: Estudio de los trastornos del aprendizaje infantil, electrofisiología de la atención y de la memoria. Dirección postal: Laboratorio de Neurometría. Edificio UIICSE, FES Iztacala, UNAM Ave. de los Barrios No. 1, Col. Los Reyes Iztacala, Tlalnepantla, Estado de México, C.P. 54090, México. Correo: jbernal@unam.mx

Creative Commons License Todo el contenido de esta revista, excepto dónde está identificado, está bajo una Licencia Creative Commons