SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.11 número22Mecanismos de transferencia de tecnología como elementos del fortalecimiento del conocimiento acumulado en la industria biofarmacéutica mexicana: El Caso de la UDIBI - IPNAnálisis de las Habilidades Administrativas y de Gestión en las Micro y Pequeñas Empresas de la ciudad de Chihuahua índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Revista

Articulo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • No hay artículos similaresSimilares en SciELO

Compartir


Nova scientia

versión On-line ISSN 2007-0705

Nova scientia vol.11 no.22 León may. 2019

https://doi.org/10.21640/ns.v11i22.1680 

Ciencias Humanas y Sociales

Psychometric properties of the Desensitization to Violence Scale for Adolescents

Propiedades psicométricas de la Escala de Desensibilización a la Violencia para Adolescentes

Jaime Sebastián F. Galán Jiménez1  * 

Omar Sánchez-Armáss Cappello1 

Luis Felipe García y Barragán2 

1 Universidad Autónoma de San Luis Potosí, San Luis Potosí, México.

2 Universidad de Guanajuato, Campus León, León, México.


Abstract

Introduction:

Desensitization to violence is the result of exposure to violence. It reduces the negative emotions in cognitive and physiological responses to violence and can even generate a positive response to it.

Method:

A mixed sequential method yielding a transactional analytic design for Exploratory Factor Analysis and Confirmatory Factor Analysis with a sample of 1720 participants of 25 different high schools and a juvenile detention center. Followed by convergent validity (as a criterion validity) with a different sample.

Results:

The EFA’s cumulative explained variance was 52% with a GFI of .98 with a three-factor model: sensitivity to violence, amusement with physical violence, and enjoyment of psychological violence. The CFA showed scores above .50 in Average Explained Variance in every factor, and an ideal model fit in every measure (CFI, AGFI, RMSEA, SRMR, and ECVI). The remaining factors are only those related with enjoying or amusement with violent behavior, indicating that the desensitization to violence is related not only with the normalization and legitimation of violence, but the increasing of the performance and amusement of it.

Discussion or Conclusion:

The scale of desensitization to violence for adolescents has adequate psychometric properties and can be a valuable instrument to generate intervention or prevention programs, especially for its intimate relationship with high scores in people interned because of their criminal behavior.

Keywords: habituation; youth; psychometrics; social psychology; violence

Resumen

Introducción:

La desensibilización a la violencia deviene de la exposición a la violencia. Reduce las emociones negativas en las respuestas cognitivas y fisiológicas hacia la violencia, incluso puede generar reacciones positivas ante ella.

Método.

Se empleó un método secuencial derivativo para finalmente realizar un diseño analítico transaccional para el análisis factorial exploratorio y confirmatorio, además se realizó una aplicación para obtener validez de criterio (convergente) con una muestra distinta. Se contó con 1720 participantes de 25 instituciones de educación superior distintas y un centro de internamiento juvenil.

Resultados.

El análisis factorial exploratorio muestra una varianza acumulada del 52% y un GFI de .98 con tres factores: sensibilidad hacia la violencia, entretenimiento con violencia física y agrado hacia la violencia psicológica. El análisis factorial confirmatorio presentó una varianza extraída superior a .50 y las propiedades psicométricas ideales en cada elemento del modelo en cada medida (CFI, AGFI, RMSEA, SRMR y ECVI). Los factores que permanecieron son aquellos relacionados con la diversión y entretenimiento con la violencia. Los cual indica que la desensibilización a la violencia no solo se relaciona con la normalización y legitimación de la violencia sino con el incremento de la realización y el agrado hacia ella.

Discusión o Conclusión:

La escala de desensibilización a la violencia para adolescentes tiene adecuadas propiedades psicométricas y puede ser un instrumento valioso para generar campañas de intervención o prevención, especialmente por su íntima relación con altos puntajes en las personas internadas a consecuencia de comportamiento criminal.

Palabras clave: habituación; juventud; psicometría; psicología social; violencia

Introduction

Exposure to violence in Mexico has reached record levels. The Mexican National Institute of Statistics and Geography (INEGI, Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Geografía, 2015) reported at least one victim of violence in 33.2% of households in the country, and 92.8% of all crimes are not reported. Mexico occupied the 144th place out of 163 countries in the 2015 Global Peace Index and recently ranked up to 140th (Institute for Economics and Peace, 2015, 2018a). However, the economic impact of violence in Mexico represented 21% of the GDP in 2017 (Institute for Economics and Peace, 2018b). INEGI (2015) also reported that there are 25,873 crime victims per each 100,000 people only in the state of San Luis Potosí. Every crime involves an act of violence, whether psychological, sexual, or physical. In the literature, exposure to violence and its desensitization are usually evaluated through indirect measures, but, to the best of our knowledge, no direct measures are reported in the literature. A theoretical framework and the development of a measure of desensitization to violence can lead to a better understanding of youth perception towards violence and its consequences.

Violence is an act that limits and breaks apart people's wills by causing physical or psychological harm. It operates in accordance with the historical moment and context, depends on the values of the society in which it takes place, and is not always obvious. Violence can be lived or witnessed, also, it can happen in different contexts such as social, couple, family, school, community, and home among others (Barrios & Paravic, 2009; Cuevas & Castro, 2009; Salazar, 2011; World Health Organization, 2014).

Cooley-Strickland, Quille, Griffin, Stuart, Bradshaw and Furr-Holden (2011) propose that living in violent communities is harmful, especially for children and adolescents. Furthermore, the victims of indirect violence are more numerous than those of direct violence, and chronic community violence affects its members in the long term.

Given that social learning theory considers that people's beliefs and perceptions are affected by the environment, it was used in this study to address the phenomenon of desensitization to violence. The direct or indirect experience of social interaction can generate behavior models, actions that become learning standards and frame the events experienced by people and interpreted by social labeling. This process depends on the frequency with which an event occurs (e.g., violence) and the ideas about the event that could later become an accepted social model, or even a normative belief. Likewise, according to social learning, desensitization to violence is partly the result of a normative belief that allows for the exercise of violence without acknowledgment as such (Akers, 2009; Bandura, 1971, 1973; Ghiso, 2012; Huesmann & Guerra, 1997). According to Akers and Jennings (2016), criminal and violent behavior is learned as a communication process, mainly among people that are in the immediate context. Learning includes techniques with specific motives, goals, attitudes and rationalizations. A person commits a crime of violence because his ideas in favor overcome those that are against.

The aim of this paper is to report the psychometric properties of a desensitization to violence scale for adolescents. Our hypothesis was that the Desensitization to Violence Scale is reliable and valid. For that purpose content, construct (exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis) and criteria validity were evaluated.

Desensitization to violence

Wolpe (1971) proposed a desensitization technique aimed at the reduction of phobic reactions, among other emotional responses; however, the main concepts in the present investigation on desensitization to violence derive from a different construct. In this study, our perspective on the attribute of desensitization is closer to the one espoused by Funk, Baldacci, Pasold and Baumgardner (2004). These authors suggest that desensitization to violence originates from a constant exposure to violence, which in turn creates emotional desensitization through diminished emotional responses. Constant exposure also creates cognitive desensitization by highlighting the idea that violence is inevitable and mundane. These two, emotional and cognitive desensitization, decrease the possibility to censor violence.

Galan and Preciado (2014) summarize the construct of desensitization to violence follows:

Desensitization to violence is a process that occurs as a result of the exposure to violence either in the media or in real life. Furthermore, the phenomenon can be recognized in the reduction of physiological, emotional, cognitive or psychological responses, as well as the perception of, or attention given to violent acts that were experienced or witnessed. In conclusion, desensitization to violence seems to produce the belief that violence is trivial and inevitable, even capable of generating positive emotions (p. 70).

Recently, Galán and Sánchez-Armáss (2018) found a significant correlation between desensitization to violence and age, and a medium to high effect size in contrast to groups of mature and older adults. They found more desensitization in a city with greater violence index. Alimardani, Ardekani and Astany (2014) reviewed the concept of desensitization to violence and related it to media and neighborhoods; they report that being part of a violent environment results in desensitization to violence due to exposure. Diaz, Rivera and Reyes (2012) performed studies about anxiety with inhabitants of the crime-ridden city of Ciudad Juárez, Mexico. They claim that six months after violence started in the city there was a significant increase in the population’s anxiety levels; after two years, the difference had disappeared and people had grown accustomed to violence.

Krahe, Möller, Kirwil, Huesmann, Felber and Berger (2011) address the issue of desensitization to violence as result of interaction with the media. They found that desensitization increases the interest for violent scenarios. They relate desensitization with difficulty to recognize violent lexicon, and the relation was associated with normative beliefs about violence. Mrug and Windle (2010) had previously pointed out the presence of cognitive schemata related with aggression and decreased empathy. Building on these findings, Galán (2018) studied violence exposure and desensitization discourse. Results suggested that amusement towards violence was an identifying characteristic of those who did not recognize and were unable to label violence in their daily lives. Mrug, Madan, Cook and Wright (2014) compare the effects of media violence and real-life violence on emotional and physiological desensitization. Studying a community sample of youths, they found that elevated levels of real life violence have more prominent effects on adolescents’ emotions than media violence, with outcomes including decreased empathy and trauma symptoms. On the other hand, limited exposure to violence could result in higher empathy and decreases distress from repeated scenes of violence.

Instruments measuring similar constructs of desensitization to violence

Megias, Romero-Sanchez, Duran, Moya and Bohner (2013) developed a scale of acceptance to modern myths about sexual aggression. Their instrument has 30 items, and it covers myths related to sexual violence. It focuses on cognitions about social interaction, sexual harassment, rape, and sexual interaction. However, its psychometric properties were found to be unclear.

Corey (2009) presented the Violence Sensitivity Scale (VSS) as a dissertation at the University of Rhode Island. A sample of 169 participants simultaneously completed the Nonviolence Test (NVT). To validate the VSS, Corey used the Attitudes Toward Violence Scale (ATVS) and the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale (MCSD). The VSS measures exposure to violence, attitudes, social desirability, and non-violence. The author addresses the concept of desensitization to violence, which is similar to our construct. He made a principal components factor analysis, in which nonviolence and social desirability were found to predict sensitivity to violence. Corey’s instrument presented a low total variance explained and the sample population to validate the scale could be considered insufficient, according to Nunally (1979) and DeVellis (2012). Moreover, it should be pointed that the only source in which his scale can be found is in his doctoral thesis, not in a peer-reviewed article.

Galdames and Aron (2007) carried out similar research presenting a scale that measures violence legitimizing beliefs. The authors conducted their study using a representative sample (sample a) which was compared with a group of children (sample b) identified as exposed to violence. They found five factors: legitimacy as fair defense, as a parenting strategy, to control the partner, to resolve conflicts, and as difficulties controlling emotions. The scale has 33 Likert-type items, grouped into four subscales, which refer to physical violence among peers (boys), physical violence among peers (girls), parent-child physical violence, and physical and verbal violence in the couple (parents). It has a Cronbach's alpha of 0.86, and uses a sample of 608 participants and Varimax rotation, which explained only 34.7% of total variance.

What follows is a review of the most recent studies found reporting desensitization to violence measures. Tarabah, Kurdahi, Usta and Doyle (2015) used the Media Preference survey for exposure to violence, and the KID-SAVE survey and Attitudes Toward Violence Scale Child Version to assess desensitization to violence. Dushyant and Wiesner (2015) used the Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance Survey to measure exposure to violence, including six adapted items from the Major Depressive Disorder Subscale of the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children to evaluate depressive symptoms. Other researchers have different measured psychological aspects with different instruments, for instance, witnessing community violence, depressive symptoms, and anxiety symptoms (Gaylord-Harden, Dickson, & Pierre, 2015). Using the Child Behavior Checklist, Fleckman, Drury, Taylor and Theall (2016) studied externalizing behaviors, and they used the Conflict Tactic Scale to assess direct and indirect exposure to violence. Read, Ballard, Emery and Bazzini (2016) examined arousal and desensitization to violence as a result of playing violent videogames by assessing progressive muscle relaxation and facial electromyography data, among other factors, and they also used the Self-Assessment Manikin, the Game Experience Questionnaire, and the Demographics and Exposure Questionnaire as self-reported input for their study.

Regarding studies on media and desensitization, we reviewed a paper by Funk (2015) describing physical and psychological measures of desensitization to violence, but none of them used a direct approach to measure the phenomenon. To the best of our knowledge, there is no instrument to date to directly measure desensitization to violence, and the existing instruments fail to address more than one of the several components or factors explained by our instrument, which makes our contribution relevant and innovative.

Method

A mixed sequential method yielding a transactional analytic design for Exploratory Factor Analysis and Confirmatory Factor Analysis. Followed by a criterion validity.

Participants

Main Sample. Sampling was conducted in three stages: First, 25 schools were randomly selected from a total of 148 listed in the cities of San Luis Potosi and Soledad de Graciano Sanchez. Secondly, classrooms were used as clusters, considering the possibilities, timing, and logistics that each institution considered pertinent. Finally, we also recruited adolescent volunteers at the local Juvenile Detention Center.

The initial number of participants was 1988. We discarded participants with large unanswered sections, those that clearly responded without reading, and those outside the age range of 15 to18. The final sample consisted of 1720 participants, 869 (50.5%) females and 851 (49.5%) males. A total of 633 (36.8%) participants were 15 years old, 467 (27.2%) were 16 years old, 466 (27.1%) were 17 years old and 154 (9%) were 18 years old. Regarding their high school year, 861 (50.1%) were in the first year, 447 (27%) on the second year, and 396 (23%) were in the third year. At the time of the study, there were 19 inmate adolescents at the Juvenile Detention Center. Sixteen volunteered to participate in the study, comprising 0.9% of the total final sample.

Some urban areas in both cities are classified as “polygons of violence”, i.e., territorial units that are selected based on their criminal characteristics. Thirty nine point four percent of the sample (n=677) lived inside one of these violence polygons, 716 (41.6%) lived in an adjacent area, and 290 (16.9%) lived away from a polygon of violence. The majority of the sample (74%) reported living with both of parents, and 62% declared practicing a sport. Only 39% of subjects reported participating in artistic activities, and quite unexpectedly, only 33% reported suffering from economic deprivation, despite the fact that scarce economic resources characterize sampled neighborhoods.

Secondary Sample. A convenience sample of 300 adults volunteers was recruited to evaluate convergent validity. Thirty nine point seven percent of the sample were male (n=119) and 181 (60.3%) were female. The average age was 31.56 (SD=2.88) years. Ten percent of the sample reported having been arrested.

Procedure

The preliminary instrument was sent to 14 experts, but only eight responded. The inclusion criteria guiding the selection of these experts was based on their expertise in the fields of violence or psychometrics, or preferably in both. Experts were recruited from different Mexican states including, San Luis Potosí, Mexico City, Sinaloa, Monterrey, and Guanajuato, as well as from the Spanish Balearic Islands. The semi-structured interviews in Galan (2016) suggested the following categories: violence as entertainment, violence as normal or common, and violence as trivial or unimportant. After the in-depth interviews (Galan, 2018), we added these new categories: enjoyment of violence, unnoticed violence, decreased support, violence as a form of hierarchy, justified violence, and violence as a requirement. These categories led to 100 items, which were sent to experts in order to assess content validity. After the content validity statistics, we dropped 18 items, 59 were retained, and 23 were modified. These modifications were based on an article by Merino and Livia (2009), which recommends a 0.7 value for Aiken’s V validation coefficient, which was the criterion to retain the item. We started with an Aiken’s V coefficient of .75, then we dropped the low score items we got .78 with a confidence interval with a lower limit of .63 and an upper limit of .91. The judges could assign a score in a range between 1 and 5; the most appropriate concordance coefficient to evaluate the agreement between evaluators is, according to Escobar-Pérez and Cuervo-Martínez (2008), Kendall’s W. This is illustrated in Table 1. Following Dubé’s proposed guidelines, an average of .26 is considered "fair," between .21 - .40.

Table 1 Different CFA model fit measures of the desensitization to violence scale 

Model X2 Cmin/df p SRMR GFI AGFI TLI CFI Pclose RMSEA ECVI
Initial 843.88 4.71 0.01 0.5 0.92 0.9 0.88 0.89 0.01 0.06 1
four factors 260.24 3.71 0.01 0.03 0.96 0.94 0.95 0.96 0.18 0.05 0.34
three factors 85.099 2.83 0.01 0.026 0.98 0.96 0.97 0.98 0.8 0.04 0.14
Last model 19.85 1.24 0.22 0.01 0.99 0.98 0.99 0.99 0.99 0.01 0.06
Unifactorial 376.87 11.77 0.01 0.06 0.92 0.87 0.86 0.9 0.01 0.1 0.44

We also added items following the judges’ suggestion, and at last we were left with a total of 84 items. Of that total items we proceed to exclude 24 with reliability coefficients below .3. We then proceed to run an EFA with the theoretical dimensions of: desensitization to violence in psychological, physical and sexual manifestations, also desensitization to exert violence, desensitization to witness violence that includes, as a second order factor, normative beliefs, prosocial and the implied amusement with violence.

Statistics analysis. We carried out a missing data imputation using a multivariate imputation by chained equations (MICE) software package (Van Buuren & Grothouis-Oudshoorn, 2011). The sample was randomly split into two subgroups; an 876-participant subgroup was used for EFA by means of Factor software (version 10.5), and the other subgroup, including 847 participants, was used for CFA by AMOS software (version 21.0).

The EFA was performed using unweighted least squares and a promin rotation with a polychorical correlation matrix, following the recommendations of Lloret-Segura, Ferreres-Traver, Hernández-Baeza and Tomás-Marco (2014). We ran a parallel analysis with optimal implementation (Timmerman & Lorenzo-Seva, 2011) to determinate the number of ideal dimensions and we excluded items with loadings smaller than .4 and those loadings in two factors above .4. We evaluated the model’s characteristics using Goodness of Fit Index (GFI), Comparative Fit Index (CFI), reliability measures and Mardia’s multivariate asymmetry skewness and kurtosis (Mardia, 1970) measures, among others.

For the CFA we ran the structural model using maximum likelihood rotation and assessed construct validity GFI >.90, CFI > .90, RMSEA <.08 and Cmin/DF < 5. Once we got these characteristics evaluated the scale’s convergent validity using Average Variance Extracted (AVE) >.5 in every dimension and finally discriminant validity by having all the correlation between these constructs below 0.85. All these moments imply a Compose Reliability (CR) >.7 in every dimension (Asyraf, 2013). To obtain the criterion validity evidence we use a Pearson correlation between the total of both scales.

Instrument. We administered the Desensitization to Violence Scale for adults (F. Galan, Hernandez, Martínez and Aguilar, in press) \and the Desensitization to Violence Scale for adolescents.

Ethics. The present study adhered to the Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct (American Psychological Association, 2017), which according to section 8.01 states that institutions are free to allow a study if they deem it adequate. Also, based on section 8.05 (about anonymity and harmless research), and section 8.8 (debriefing) the chance to sustain communication with the investigators was offered, and it was clarified that participation was completely voluntary.

Results

An EFA was conducted with the remaining 84 items (excluding those with low correlation item-total <.3), using Unweighted Least Squares (ULS) as method for extraction and Normalized Weighted Oblimin (Lorenzo-Seva, 2000) for rotation to achieve factor simplicity.

In the first rotation, following Mardia’s criterion and Muthen and Kaplan’s (1992) recommendations, we used a polychoric correlation matrix and in the process we excluded 63 items that either loaded on two factors (>.4) or had poor loadings (<.4). The results lead us to reduce the model to 5 dimensions: (1) diversion to exert psychological violence, (2) amusement to witness physical violence, (3) amusement to witness psychological violence, (4) prosociality and normalization, and (5) justification of violence.

The last EFA model resulted in good characteristics with 21 items. The model explained 73% of the cumulative variance and 46.79% with the first factor. Determinant matrix = 0.00 as well as a Bartlett's statistic = 64.36.7 df = 210; p < 0.000 and a Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO)=.89, which were considered satisfactory. In order to determine the number of dimensions we used the minimum average partial test (Velicer, 1976) that suggested three factors. Goodness of fit index (GFI) was .99, the reliability was estimated with Cronbach’s Alpha, the first factor score was .80, .82 for the second, .83 for the third, .77 and the last .61: normalization of violence, status, justification and enjoyment with violence and diversion to exert psychological violence.

Results of the CFA

We performed a CFA with the remaining five dimensions using maximum likelihood considering modification indices and all implied moments. The aim was to get the ideal characteristics and evidences of validity: construct, discriminant and convergent validity in the CFA model. The different models with the exact features of the CFA are described in the Table 1.

The first model has acceptable features but some of them need to be improved, we proceed to apply the modification indices, and to erase the items in the factor of prosociality. In the model with four factors we can almost see all the desired properties, however, the p value was not achieved. We then proceeded to eliminate the Legitimation and Normalization of Violence factor because of the standardized residual covariances and validity concerns. The three dimensions model presented almost all the desired psychometric properties, even validity and reliability, but the expected p-value for the model was still short. The final model lost two items to achieve the desired psychometric properties. We proceeded to evaluate the model’s validity using the Average Variance Extracted (AVE) and the Composite Reliability (CR). Table 2 reports validity and reliability of the final model with and without the two items.

Table 2 Psychometric properties of the desensitization to violence scale in the final and previous model 

Final model Model without p value
Psychometric properties Diversion to exert psychological violence (1) Amusement to witness physical violence (2) Amusement to witness psychological violence (3) 1 2 3
Composite Reliability (CR) .82 .68 .70 .82 .78 .80
Average Variance Extracted (AVE) .61 .52 .44 .61 .55 .50
Maximum Shared Variance (MSV) .51 .33 .51 .45 .26 .45

With the final model we proceed to test if there were any collinearity difficulties and all the VIF appeared lower than 3.0 between the different possibilities among the factors pointing that there were no method bias, also confirmed in the model features in the unifactorial solution. We present in Figure 1 and 2 the two last remaining models.

Figure 1 Confirmatory Factor Analysis model for the desensitization to violence scale model without p value 

Figure 2 Final model of the desensitization to violence scale 

Results of criterion validity (third sample). Table 3 showcases the convergent validity of the Desensitization to Violence Scale which reveals that validity is satisfactory, with r=.732 p<.001 in both total of construct scores. Whit the previous model in Figure 1.

Table 3 Convergent validity between the desensitization to violence scales for adolescents and desensitization to violence scale for adults 

Factor Normalization and legitimation of violence Enjoyment with violence Total desensitization to violence in adolescents
Enjoyment in exerting psychological violence .360 .630 .872
Amusement in witnessing physical violence .514 .571 .912
Amusement in witnessing psychological violence .695 .499 .765
Total desensitization to violence scale for adolescents score .731 .954 .732

Pearson’s correlation significant at a 0.01 level (bilateral)

Discussion

The Desensitization to Violence Scale’s psychometric performance shows that it is a valid measure. The methodological contribution is relevant, since research on desensitization to violence has been limited to clinical or experimental experiences, or to instruments that do not measure the construct of desensitization to violence directly. Only two other instruments have similar themes. The first is Galdames and Aaron’s (2007) scale, which addresses violence legitimation beliefs but does not focus on adolescents; it properly addresses desensitization to violence, but the measurement is based on only one factor called "legitimacy as a fair defense", which could be similar to our scale’s component of "amusement with physical violence”. The other instrument is the one proposed by Corey (2009) the Sensitization to Violence Scale, which is described only in his doctoral thesis. The explained variance did not exceed 35%, and its factors (exposure to violence, attitudes, social desirability, and nonviolence) are not properly woven into the construct of desensitization to violence, although that is mentioned in the thesis. Therefore, the Desensitization to Violence Scale assessed can be considered a valid scale that offers an original vision. Many of its factors are based on Funk et al., (2004), who claim that desensitization to violence also increases pro-violent attitudes.

Regarding convergent validity, although the factors in both scales are not the same and the target population is also different, even with different questions and factors the scale obtained a remarkable score. It is crucial to highlight that the factor of legitimation and normalization of violence in the desensitization to violence scale for adults correlate near r=.7 with the desensitization to violence scale for adolescents. This implies that the factor validity could get diminished by the redundant measure, which means that these could be an aging characteristic.

The reliability and validity of the Desensitization to Violence Scale are adequate for measuring the violent phenomena currently taking place within the Mexican society. As Akers (2016) mentions, violence could lead the whole of society to increase crime rates, naturalize and accept the violent behavior as normative, and even generate positive responses towards it.

The field of social psychology could use the instrument to reveal more potential consequences of exposure to violence, along with its implications, such as increasing the acceptance of violence, thoughts of violent behavior, positive emotions toward violence, decreased sympathy and assistance to victims, and anxiety, as well as the emotional, cognitive, and psychological responses to violent events, attention to violence, possibility of violent behavior, and the effects on prosocial behavior (Galan & Preciado, 2014; Funk et al., 2004; Krahe, Möller, Kirwil, Huesman, Felber, & Berger, 2011; Su, Mrug & Windle, 2010; Staude-Müller, Bliesener, & Luthman, 2008).

Conclusion

The Desensitization to Violence Scale for adolescents has shown to possess all the required evidence of validity for the model. The factors measure an innovative construct that could lead to an accurate and reliable measure to revert, prevent and improve social programs and clinical interventions in the early ages. This instrument is expected to help understand, prevent and generate tools to verify the effectiveness of the efforts to reduce the increasing violence and the indifference or normalization that can result from exposure to real violent situations. Limitations: we need to compare if the remaining EFA and CFA with a gender approach and also study the relationship with exposure to violence, classification by context.

Acknowledgment

To Victoria Aurora Ferrer Pérez that was very important to continue this research and the institutions and centers that accepted to receive our research.

References

Akers, Ronald. (2009). Applications of social learning principles. Some programs for the treatment and prevention of crime. Available at: http://uned-illesbalears.net/Tablas/serrano3.pdfLinks ]

Akers, Roland L. & Jennings, Wesley G. (2016). Social Learning Theory. In Piquero, A. The handbook of the criminological theory. Oxford: John Wiley and Sons, Inc. [ Links ]

American Psychological Association (2010) Ethic code. Reviewed in: http://www.apa.org/ethics/code/index.aspxLinks ]

Alimardani, Armin., Ardekani, Ali. & Astany, Leila (2014). A Bio-Social Review to Mitigate the Punishment of Unwanted Acts. International journal of criminology and sociology, 3: 348-359. [ Links ]

Asyraf, Wan Mohamad. (2013). A Comparison Of Partial Least Square Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM) and Covariance Based Structural Equation Modeling (CB-SEM) for Confirmatory Factor Analysis. International Journal of Engineering Science and Innovative Technology, 2 (5), 198-205. [ Links ]

Awang, Zainudin., Afthanorhand, Asyraf. & Asri, M. A. M. (2015). Parametric and Non Parametric Approach in Structural Equation Modeling (SEM): The Application of Bootstrapping. Modern Applied Science, 9 (9), 58-67. [ Links ]

Bandura, Albert. (1973) Aggression a social learning analysis. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall [ Links ]

Bandura, Albert. (1977) Social learning theory. United States of America: Prentice-Hall [ Links ]

Cooley-Strickland, Michele, Quille, Tanya J., Griffin, Robert S., Stuart, Elizabet. A., Bradshaw, Catherine P. & Furr-Holden, Debra. (2011) Effects of youth’s exposure to community violence: the more project. Psychosocial Intervention, 20 (2): 131-148. [ Links ]

Corey, James. (2009). A concurrent validation of the violence sensitivity scale. University of Rhode Island. Doctoral Dissertation. [ Links ]

Cuevas, María Clara. & Castro, Leonidas. (2009). Emotional and behavioral effects of exposure to violence in children and adolescents in Colombia. Behavioral Psychology / Psicología Conductual, 17 (2):277-297. [ Links ]

DeVellis, Robert. F. (2012). Scale development theory and applications. United States of America: Sage. [ Links ]

Dushyant, Kathan. & Wiesner, Margit. (2015) Direct and Indirect Violence Exposure: Relations to Depression for Economically Disadvantaged Ethnic Minority Mid-Adolescents. Violence and Victims, 30 (1): 120-135. http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/0886-6708.VV-D-12-00042 [ Links ]

Escobar-Pérez, Jazmín. & Cuervo-Martínez, Ángela. (2008). Validez de contenido y juicio de expertos: una aproximación a su utilización. Avances en medición, 65, 27-36. 10.1177/0886260515584337 [ Links ]

F. Galán, Jaime Sebastián. (2016). Exposición y desensibilización a la violencia, vivencias y creencias de adolescentes. En Santana, Raúl Armando y Aguayo, Adriana (Coords.) Retratos psicosociales en el México contemporáneo, pp. 57-70. México: Universidad Enrique Díaz de León. [ Links ]

F. Galán, Jaime Sebastián. (2018). Exposición a la violencia en adolescentes: desensibilización, legitimación y naturalización. Diversitas: perspectivas en psicología, 14 (1), 55-67. [ Links ]

F. Galán, Jaime Sebastián. & Sánchez-Armáss, Omar. (2018). Older adults are right: young people are more desensitized to violence. MOJ Gerontology & Geriatrics, 3 (3), 225-227. DOI: 10.15406/mojgg.2018.03.00120 [ Links ]

F. Galán, Jaime Sebastián., Hernández, Noemí. A., Martínez, César. M. & Aguilar, Yolanda. (in press) Desensitization to violence scale for adults. Manuscript in preparation. [ Links ]

Fanti, K. A., Vanman, E., Henrich, C. C. & Avraamides, M.N. (2009) Desensitization to media violence over a short period of time. Aggressive behavior, 35 (2): 179-187. [ Links ]

Fleckman, Julia., Drury, Stacy S., Taylor, Catherine A., & Theall, Katherine P. (2016) Role of Direct and Indirect Violence Exposure on Externalizing Behavior in Children. Journal of Urban Health: Bulletin of the New York Academy of Medicine, 93 (3): 479-492. doi:10.1007/s11524-016-0052- [ Links ]

Funk, Jeanne B., Baldacci, Heidi Bechtoldt., Pasold, Tracie., & Baumgardner, Jennifer. (2004). Violence exposure in real life, video games, television, movies, and the Internet: Is there desensitization? Journal of Adolescence, 27: 23-39. [ Links ]

Funk, Jeanne B. (2015). Playing Violent Video Games and Desensitization to Violence. Child & adolescent psychiatric clinics, 24 (1), 65-77. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chc.2014.08.001 [ Links ]

Galdames, Susan, & Arón, Ana María. (2007). Construcción de una escala para medir creencias legitimadoras de violencia en población infantil. Psykhe, 16 (1): 15-25. [ Links ]

Galán, Jaime Sebastian. & Preciado, María de Lourdes (2014) Violence desensitization: delimitating a construct through a theoretical revision. Uaricha, 11 (25), 70-81. [ Links ]

Gaylord-Harden, N. K., Cunningham, J. A. & Zelenik, B. (2011) Effects of exposure to community violence on internalizing symptoms: does desensitization to violence occur in African American youth? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39: 711-719. DOI 10.1007/s10802-011-9510-x [ Links ]

Gaylord-Harden, N. K., Dickson, D. & Pierre, C. (2015) Profiles of Community Violence Exposure Among African American Youth: An Examination of Desensitization to Violence Using Latent Class Analysis. Journal of interpersonal violence, 31(11): 1-25. DOI: 10.1177/0886260515572474 [ Links ]

Ghiso, Alfredo Manuel. (2012). Algunos límites de las respuestas frente a la violencia y la inseguridad, en las instituciones educativas. Revista Latinoamericana de Ciencias Sociales, 10, 2: 815-824. [ Links ]

Hernández, Roberto., Fernández, Carlos. & Baptista, M. Pilar. (2014) Metodología de la investigación. México: McGrawHill. [ Links ]

Huesmann, L. Rowell y Guerra, Nancy. (1997). Children's normative beliefs about aggression and aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 2: 408-419. [ Links ]

Institute for Economics & Peace (2015) Global Peace Index 2015: Measuring peace, its causes and its economic value. Sydney: Institute for Economics & Peace. Retrieved from: http://economicsandpeace.org/wp-content/uploads/2015/06/Global-Peace-Index-Report-2015_0.pdfLinks ]

Institute for Economics & Peace (2018a) Global Peace Index 2018: Measuring peace in a complex world. Sydney: Institute for Economics & Peace. Retrieved from: http://visionofhumanity.org/app/uploads/2018/06/Global-Peace-Index-2018-2.pdfLinks ]

Institute for Economics & Peace (2018b) Mexico Peace Index 2018: Mapping the evolution of peace and its drivers. Sydney: Institute for Economics & Peace. Retrieved from: http://visionofhumanity.org/app/uploads/2017/04/Mexico-Peace-Index-2018-English-003.pdfLinks ]

Instituto Nacional de Estadística y Geografía (2015) Encueta nacional de victimización y percepción sobre seguridad pública. Retrieved from: http://www.beta.inegi.org.mx/contenidos/proyectos/enchogares/regulares/envipe/2016/doc/envipe2016_slp.pdfLinks ]

Kelley, T. L. (1935). Essential traits of mental life: The purposes and principles underlying the election and measurement of independent mental factors, together with computational tables. Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. [ Links ]

Krahe, Barbara., Möller, Ingrid., Kirwil, Lucina., Huesmann, L. Rowell., Felber, Juliane. & Berger, Anja. (2011) Desensitization to Media Violence: Links With Habitual Media Violence Exposure, Aggressive Cognitions, and Aggressive Behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 100, 4: 630-64 [ Links ]

Lloret-Segura, S., Ferreres-Traver, A., Hérnandez-Baeza & Tomás-Marco, I. (2014). El análisis factorial exploratorio de los ítems: una guía práctica, revisada y actualizada. Anales de psicología, 30 (3), 1151-1169. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.6018/analesps.30.3.199361 [ Links ]

Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2000). The weighted oblimin rotation. Psychometrika, 65 (3): 301-318. [ Links ]

Lorenzo-Seva, U. & Ferrando, P. J. (2015). Factor 10.3. Spain: Rovira I Virgili University. [ Links ]

Megias, J. L., Romero-Sánchez, M., Duran, M., Moya, M., &Bohner, G. (2013). Acceptance of Modern Myths About Sexual Aggression (AMMSA). Bielefeld University. doi:10.4119/unibi/2641703. [ Links ]

Merino, César. & Livia, José. (2009) Intervalos de confianza asimétricos para el índice la validez de contenido: Un programa Visual Basic para la V de Aiken. Anales de psicología, 25 (1): 169-171. [ Links ]

Mrug, Sylvie. & Windle, Michael. (2010) Prospective effects of violence exposure across multiple contexts on early adolescents ‘internalizing and externalizing problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(8), 953-961. DOI:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02222.x [ Links ]

Mrug, Sylvie, Madan, Anjana, Cook III, Edwin. W. & Wright, Rex. A. (2014) Emotional and physiological desensitization to real-life and movie violence. Journal of youth adolescence,44(5): 1092-1108. DOI 10.1007/s10964-014-0202-z [ Links ]

Muthén, B., & Kaplan, D. (1992). A comparison of some methodologies for the factor analysis of non-normal Likert variables: A note on the size of the model. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 45, 19-30. [ Links ]

Nunnally, Jum. C. (1987). Teoría psicométrica. México: Trillas. [ Links ]

Read, Glenna.L., Ballard, Mary., Emery, Lisa. J. & Bazzini, Doris. G. (2016). Examining desensitization using facial electromyography: violent videogames, gender, and affective responding. Computers in human behavior, 62, 201 - 211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.074 [ Links ]

Salazar, Margarita. (2011). Un mal antiguo una larga historia. Noesis, 19, 38, 109- 130. [ Links ]

Staude-Müller, Frithjof, Bliesener, T. & Luthman, S. (2008) Hostile and Hardened? An Experimental Study on Desensitization to Violence and Suffering Through Playing Video Games. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 67 (1): 41-50. [ Links ]

Su, Wei M., Mrug, Sylvie. & Windle, Michele. (2010) Social cognitive and emotional mediators link violence exposure and parental nurturance to adolescent aggression. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 39 (6): 814-824. [ Links ]

Tarabah, Asma, Kurdahi, Lina., Usta, Jinan. & Doyle, John. (2015) Exposure to Violence and Children’s Desensitization Attitudes in Lebanon. Journal of Interpersonal Violence: 1 -22. [ Links ]

Timmerman, Marieke. E., & Lorenzo-Seva, Urbano. (2011) Dimensionality assessment of ordered polytomous items with parallel analysis. Psychological Methods, 16: 209-220. [ Links ]

Van Buuren, Stef. & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, Karin. (2011) Mice: Multivariate Imputation by Chained Equations in R. Journal of Statistical Software, 45 (3): 1-67. [ Links ]

Velicer, W. F. (1976). Determining the number of components from the matrix of partial correlations. Psychometrika, 41, 321-327. [ Links ]

Wolpe, Joseph (1961). The systematic desensitization treatment of neuroses. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 132: 180-203. [ Links ]

World Health Organization (2014) Global status report on violence prevention 2014. World Health Organization: Luxemburg. [ Links ]

Received: October 01, 2018; Accepted: January 15, 2019

* Autor para correspondencia: Jaime Sebastián F. Galán Jiménez, E-mail: psic.sebastiangalan@gmail.com

Creative Commons License This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License