SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.21 número2Trastorno por déficit de atención, comorbilidad y tratamiento en el Hospital Psiquiátrico Infantil Dr Juan N. Navarro, Ciudad de MéxicoEfecto de la empatía sobre el procesamiento cortical temprano y tardío de rostros índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Revista

Articulo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • No hay artículos similaresSimilares en SciELO

Compartir


Revista mexicana de neurociencia

versión On-line ISSN 2604-6180versión impresa ISSN 1665-5044

Rev. mex. neurocienc. vol.21 no.2 Ciudad de México mar./abr. 2020  Epub 22-Mar-2022

https://doi.org/10.24875/rmn.19000132 

Original articles

Electroencephalographic activity during sentence production in healthy and schizophrenic men

Actividad electroencefalográfica durante la producción de oraciones en hombres sanos y con esquizofrenia

Héctor J. Pelayo-González1  * 

Dora Granados-Ramos2 

Víctor Alcaraz-Romero2 

Verónica Reyes-Meza3 

1Neuropsychology Unit, Benemérita Universidad Autónoma de Puebla, Puebla, Mexico

2Psychobiology Laboratory, Faculty of Psychology, Universidad Veracruzana, Xalapa, Veracruz, Mexico

3Centro Tlaxcala de Biología de la Conducta, Universidad Autónoma de Tlaxcala, Tlaxcala, Mexico


Abstract

Background:

Schizophrenia is a psychiatric disorder marked by hallucinations, delusions, cognitive deficit, apathy, and thinking disorders. Speech disturbance is one of the main diagnostic criteria during acute periods of schizophrenia. In addition, abnormalities of brain language areas have been consistently described in schizophrenic individuals. The aim of this study was to describe the linguistic performance and cerebral activity during sentence production using word pairs with high or low semantic relatedness.

Material and methods:

We recorded electrical brain activity (electroencephalography [EEG]) of 15 healthy men and 11 men with disorganized schizophrenia while they were producing sentences with high and low relatedness word pairs.

Results:

The results showed significant differences; participants in both groups had significant longer latencies to produce sentences with low than with high relatedness word pairs. Furthermore, sentences formed with low relatedness pairs were significantly longer than those formed with high relatedness word pairs in both groups. EEG parameters also showed differences; in healthy subjects, we found enhancement of absolute alpha rhythm over the occipital leads during the period preceding sentence production with high relatedness word pairs.

Conclusion:

In contrast, in schizophrenia patients producing sentences with high relatedness word pairs, it was theta and beta rhythms that were enhanced in frontal regions.

Key words Semantic relatedness; Verbal fluency; Electroencephalography; Absolute power

Resumen

Antecedentes:

La esquizofrenia es una enfermedad caracterizada por alucinaciones, déficits cognitivos, apatía y alteraciones del pensamiento. Uno de los principales criterios diagnósticos durante los periodos agudos son las alteraciones del lenguaje. El objetivo de este trabajo fue describir el desempeño lingüístico y la actividad cerebral durante la producción de oraciones usando pares de palabras con alta y baja relación semántica.

Material y métodos:

Registramos la actividad eléctrica (EEG) de 15 hombres sanos y 11 pacientes con esquizofrenia desorganizada mientras producían oraciones con pares de palabras con alta y baja relación semántica.

Resultados:

Los resultados mostraron diferencias significativas; ambos grupos tuvieron latencias más largas al producir oraciones usando palabras con baja relación semántica. Además, estas oraciones fueron significativamente más largas, que aquellas formadas usando palabras con alta relación semántica, en ambos grupos. El EEG también mostró diferencias; en los sujetos sanos hubo un aumento del ritmo alfa en el occipital durante el periodo previo a la producción de oraciones con alta relación semántica.

Conclusiones:

En contraste, en los pacientes con esquizofrenia fueron los ritmos teta y beta los que incrementaron en regiones frontales al producir este tipo de oraciones.

Palabras clave Cercanía semántica; Fluidez verbal; EEG; Poder absoluto

Introduction

In patients with schizophrenia, language disorder has been considered a diagnostic indicator of the illness1. Particularly, abnormalities in connections between words and concepts are the most representative sign of thought disorder2-4.

Some studies have showed that schizophrenic patients have deficits in tasks involving semantic relations between words5,6. Building sentences with highly semantically related words lead to fast activation of their inner representation in working memory (WM)7. Sentence production involves cognitive processes that are based on the integration of activity in multiple brain regions, particularly associative cortical sites8,9.

One technique widely used to study brain mechanisms of cognitive impairments in patients with mental disorders is electroencephalography (EEG)10. EEG studies indicate that between 50% and 60% of patients with schizophrenia show slowing of alpha rhythm, although 15% of them show beta activity between 13 Hz and 18 Hz and 10% of the patients have paroxysmal EEG changes11.

Some authors have used EEG to describe the brain functioning that underlies cognitive task performance in schizophrenic patients12. Most of the symptoms are evident while patients are solving daily life problems; cognitive researchers are aware of this problem and have developed experimental conditions that simulate life demands, such as arithmetical problem-solving13; memory retrieval14; syntactical language organization, and communication fluency15.

In this study, we analyzed the EEG during sentence production in schizophrenic and healthy men. Our objective is to identify possible changes in brain electrical activity in healthy and schizophrenic subjects during the construction of sentences using word pairs with high and low semantic relatedness. We predicted that sentence production using low relatedness word pairs requires more verbal processing and WM capacity than using high relatedness word pairs, and this would be associated to differences in EEG patterns in schizophrenic patients compared to healthy men.

Methods

The study was carried out in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration and was approved by the Faculty of Psychology Institutional Review Board at the Universidad Autónoma de Puebla (Puebla, Mexico).

Participants

We analyzed the brain electrical activity of 11 men in the acute phase of disorganized schizophrenia and 15 healthy men. Patients were recruited from the Rafael Serrano Psychiatric Hospital in Puebla, México, and healthy men were selected by advertisement. All subjects were right-handed according to the Annette scale16. Mean age of healthy men was 32.5 years (standard deviation [SD] = 4.2) and 31.7 years (SD = 3.2) for schizophrenic subjects. Educational level of healthy men was 6.5 years (SD = 2.3) and schizophrenic subjects was 6.1 (SD = 1.4). Patients’ mean age of schizophrenia onset was 31.3 (SD = 0.6) and the mean duration of illness was 6.0 months (SD = 0.1).

All of the patients had between mild and moderate illnesses according to their scores on the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms17 (mean score = 16.5, SD = 12.8, range = 0.41). Patients were evaluated 1 day after they were admitted to the hospital to avoid changes in their clinical status. All participants were native, monolingual Spanish speakers who never learned any other language. No history of head trauma, substance abuse, or addiction was recorded. After a complete description of the study, written informed consent was obtained from all participants and their relatives.

Stimuli and task

Thirty pairs of highly related words (e.g., table chair) and 30 pairs of low-related words (e.g., dog lock) were collected from the previous work18. Each participant was instructed to construct a sentence using the pair of words involved without changing the order of words or modifying the word itself (e.g., chairs instead of chair). We presented 33 pairs of words for each of the two conditions: low-related and high-related words.

Experimental procedure

Each subject received the instruction that after hearing the pair of words, they must construct a sentence using the pair of words without changing them. The experimental session began by asking the participant to close his eyes. Over the next 5 min, the participants sat with their eyes closed to record resting brain activity. Each test began with the presentation of one pair of words, previously recorded, through headphones. When the subject was ready to state his sentence, he began to speak. The next pair of words was given 5-6 s after the end of sentence articulation. Pairs of high- and low-related words were presented in pseudorandom order.

EEG data acquisition and analysis

EEG was recorded with an EEG device (Nicolet Viking EEG) according to the international 10-20 system from the following leads: Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4, F7, F8, Fz, T3, T4, T5, T6, C3, C4, Cz, P3, P4, Pz O1, O2, and Oz referred to linked earlobes. The sampling rate was 128 Hz. Amplifiers had a bandpass from 0.5 Hz to 30 Hz. For analysis, cerebral electrical activity was selected during the period from the beginning of word presentation to the start of the first oral articulation in each word pair. This period was defined in EEG with physiological markers (quick mouth movement). The sentences were recorded for posterior linguistic analysis.

We eliminated those EEG epochs with movement artifacts. Electromyography and EOG derivative from the movement of the chin, mouth, and eyes was indicated and marked. The final analyzed EEG period was 120 s in all participants. We used the fast Fourier transformation to estimate the power spectra. We analyzed the absolute power EEG rhythms in the following frequencies bands: delta (1-3.5 Hz), theta (4-7.5 Hz), alpha (8-12.5 Hz), beta 1 (13-18.5 Hz), beta 2 (19-24.5), and beta 3 (25-30 Hz).

Behavioral data analysis

The analysis is based on the comparison of values between the group of healthy subjects and the group of patients with schizophrenia. The response variables were the latencies (time in seconds) and length of sentences (number of words) constructed using high and low relation word pairs. Due to the different number of subjects in each group, we used the Mann–Whitney U test to compare the latency between groups and between the two stimulus conditions (high and low word pair relatedness). For statistical analysis and graphical representations, we used the program Prism 5 for Windows (GraphPad Software Inc., San Diego, USA, 2002).

Results

In healthy subjects, the latency for constructing sentences with high relatedness word pairs was 5.1 s (± 0.37 SD) and with low relatedness pairs was 7.4 s (± 0.52 SD). Subjects with schizophrenia constructed sentences using pair of high relatedness word pairs in 6.7 s (± 0.30 SD) and low relatedness pairs in 10.1 s (± 0.97 SD).

Kruskal–Wallis (KW) test showed significant differences in the latency to produce sentences (KW = 47.75, p < 0.0001), post hoc Dunn’s multiple comparison tests showed that this difference depended only on word pair relatedness (Fig. 1).

Figure 1 Comparison of latency to produce sentences between healthy (white) and schizophrenic (black) men with high and low relatedness word pairs. Data are means ± standard deviation (n = 15 healthy and n = 11 schizophrenic men). Letters indicate significant differences. Details of statistics are given in the text. 

We counted the number of words utilized to construct sentences. Healthy subjects incorporated 5.86 (± 0.91 SD) words in sentences with high relatedness word pairs and used 7.8 words (± 0.91 S.D) to form sentences with low relatedness pairs. Subjects with schizophrenia incorporated 7 (± 1.0 S.D) words in sentences with high relatedness pairs and 10.18 words (± 1.3 SD) in sentences with low relatedness pairs.

KW test showed significant differences in the number of words to construct sentences (KW = 35.92, p < 0.0001), post hoc Dunn’s multiple comparison tests showed that this difference depended only on word pair relatedness (Fig. 2).

Figure 2 Difference in number of words used to produce sentences between healthy (white) and schizophrenic (black) men, with high and low relatedness word pairs. Data are means ± standard deviation (n = 15 healthy and n = 11 schizophrenic men). Letters indicate significant differences. Details of statistics are given in the text. 

EEG data

EEG data were compared between groups using the Mann–Whitney U-test to assess the statistical significance of the difference in the absolute power of each EEG band. During the construction of sentences with high relatedness word pairs, healthy subjects showed significant higher absolute power in delta and alpha rhythm than schizophrenic patients. However, schizophrenic patients showed significantly higher absolute power in theta, beta 1, beta 2, and beta 3 rhythms compare to healthy subjects (Fig. 3).

Figure 3 Comparison of the absolute power (Log PA) obtained during sentence production using highly related word pairs. Details of statistics are given in the text. 

During the construction of sentences with low relatedness word pairs, schizophrenic patients showed significant higher absolute power in delta, theta, and beta rhythms, while healthy subjects exhibited significantly higher alpha rhythm (Fig. 4).

Figure 4 Comparison of the absolute power (Log PA) obtained during sentence production using low relatedness word pairs. 

Topographical analysis

Construction of sentences with highly related word pairs induced a different distribution of absolute power between the control and schizophrenic groups. The absolute power of alpha rhythm was increase in healthy subjects over the occipital leads. The absolute power of theta and beta rhythms is increase over the frontal and central leads in schizophrenic patients (Fig. 5).

Figure 5 Brain mapping while producing sentences with high relatedness word pairs. 

Construction of sentences with low relatedness word pairs also induced different distribution of absolute power between the groups. In healthy subjects, we found enhancement of alpha rhythm, especially in occipital regions, as well as theta power in frontal areas. Schizophrenic patients showed an increase in absolute power in theta band in frontotemporal areas. In addition, a diffuse increase of absolute power in beta band appeared in schizophrenic patients (Fig. 6).

Figure 6 Brain mapping while producing sentences with low relatedness word pairs. 

Discussion

There is extensive literature about deficits in verbal functioning in schizophrenia, showing various abnormalities in verbal production, comprehension, and cerebral lateralization of language19. Several functional magnetic resonance imaging studies using language paradigms also show abnormal processing in verbal fluency tasks and loss of left hemisphere lateralization in the temporal and frontal lobes of patients with chronic schizophrenia20.

Some authors Ford and Mathalon21 have argued that EEG measures of coherence indicate interdependence of activity in the frontal speech production and temporal speech reception areas during speech in control subjects, but not in schizophrenia patients. These data suggest that a corollary discharge from frontal areas fails to alert the auditory cortex that they are self-generated, leading to the misattribution of inner speech to external sources.

Wynn et al.22 found no differences between schizophrenic patients and controls in the ability to form global objects from local elements, during visual integration tests. This is relevant to our tests because we asked patients and control subjects to form coherent sentences (global object) from isolated words (local elements). The cognitive effort required for that kind of tasks involves information sorting (word pairs), to generate a coherent order. Schizophrenic patients have difficulty maintaining thematic consistency. In our study, patients formed sentences with no morphological deficits, but which were thematically incoherent. To construct a sentence, using pairs of word, it is necessary to maintain the words in the short-term memory while seeking a context in which to put them, as well as constant monitoring to avoid mistakes23. Short-term memory and monitoring are functions associated with the work of the frontotemporal and frontoparietal cortex24-26.

Expression of language is a complex process and involves a distributed network in cortical regions. Our results demonstrate that when control subjects form sentences with highly related word pairs, absolute power over theta and beta bands is more expressive on frontopolar and frontal-inferior regions, respectively. Lin et al.27 suggested that theta power increases during longer latencies; furthermore, alpha-band oscillations are suggested to be closely associated with attentional selection, specifically anticipatory and temporal attention28.

In schizophrenic patients, the absolute power of the theta band increased in the frontotemporal regions in both hemispheres with an extension to the right parietal region. The beta band also increased diffusely. Doesburg et al. suggested that oscillatory synchronization and cross-frequency interactions are mechanisms of functional integration among distributed brain areas that support expressive language processing29.

Theta activity has been found over frontal regions when subjects form images during mental task performance30. We believe that a sentence forming task involves organizing images, but in more complex tasks, the images may be less structured. On the other hand, beta activity is associated with sensory information analysis and decision-making because the task implies a goal-oriented behavior. The assumption behind this idea is that the motor system initiates active movement preparation on the reception of stimulus input31. Language production in schizophrenia is disordered and filled with irrelevant pieces of information and derailments. Such erratic discourse may be linked to the inability to use pragmatic rules, attention, action planning, ordering, and sequencing32.

Finally, we consider that sentence production using high and low relatedness word pairs could be used to explore different cognitive process, such attention, WM, executive functions, as well as the search for coherence. We believe that in the area of neuropsychological rehabilitation, particularly for schizophrenic patients, it is necessary to create methods to solve problems in discourse construction, looking into different cognitive processes that are required for generating meaning in patients’ speech.

Conclusion

There are significant differences in behavior and EEG activity during sentences production, using low and high relatedness words. Specifically, absolute power is increased in patients with schizophrenia over the theta, and beta rythms. We consider that producing sentences with high or low relatedness words could be used for new experimental paradigms.

Differences in EEG patterns in schizophrenic patients compared to healthy men.

REFERENCES

1. Bleuler E. Dementia Praecox or the Group of Schizophrenias. Oxford:International Universities;1950. [ Links ]

2. Andreasen NC. Thought, language, and communication disorders. I. Clinical assessment, definition of terms, and evaluation of their reliability. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1979;36:1315-21. [ Links ]

3. Gattaz WF, Mayer S, Ziegler P, Platz M, Gasser T. Hypofrontality on topographic EEG in schizophrenia. Correlations with neuropsychological and psychopathological parameters. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1992;241:328-32. [ Links ]

4. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V). 5th ed. Washington:American Psychiatric Association;2015. [ Links ]

5. Manschreck TC, Maher BA. Semantic priming in thought disorders and schizophrenic patients. Schizophr Res. 1988;1 61-6. [ Links ]

6. Neely JH. Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition:a selective review of current findings and theories. In:Besner D, Humphreys GW, editors. Basic Processes in Reading and Visual Word Recognition. New York:Routledge;2012. [ Links ]

7. Pulvermüller F. The Neuroscience of Language:on Brain Circuits of Words and Serial Order. Cambridge:Cambridge University Press;2003. [ Links ]

8. Picton TW. The P300 wave of the human event-related potential. J Clin Neurophysiol. 1992;9:456-79. [ Links ]

9. Johnson R Jr. On the neural generators of the P300 component of the event-related potential. Psychophysiology. 1993;30:90-7. [ Links ]

10. Rugg M, Coles MG. Electrophysiology of Mind:event Related Brain Potentials and Cognition. Oxford:Oxford University Press;1995. [ Links ]

11. Hughes JR, John ER. Conventional and quantitative electroencephalography in psychiatry. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1999;11: 190-208. [ Links ]

12. van der Stelt O, Belger A. Application of electroencephalography to the study of cognitive and brain functions in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2007;33:955-70. [ Links ]

13. Harmony T, Fernández T, Silva J, Bernal J, Díaz-Comas L, Reyes A, et al. EEG delta activity:an indicator of attention to internal processing during performance of mental tasks. Int J Psychophysiol. 1996;24: 161-71. [ Links ]

14. Petrantonakis P, Hadjileontiadis L. Adaptive emotional information retrieval from EEG signals in the time-frequency domain. IEEE Trans Signal Process. 2012;60:132-48. [ Links ]

15. Lehmann D, Faber PL, Galderisi S, Herrmann WM, Kinoshita T, Koukkou M, et al. EEG microstate duration and syntax in acute, medication-naive, first-episode schizophrenia:a multi-center study. Psychiatry Res. 2005;138:141-56. [ Links ]

16. Annett M. A classification of hand preference by association analysis. Br J Psychol. 1970;61:303-21. [ Links ]

17. Andreasen NC. Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). Iowa:University of Iowa;1984. [ Links ]

18. González HP, Ramos DG, Romero VA. Características de la evocación lexical libre en una población mexicana adulta. Neuropsicología Latinoam. 2012;4:42-6. [ Links ]

19. DeLisi LE. Speech disorder in schizophrenia:review of the literature and exploration of its relation to the uniquely human capacity for language. Schizophr Bull. 2001;27:481-96. [ Links ]

20. Sommer IE, Ramsey NF, Kahn RS. Language lateralization in schizophrenia, an fMRI study. Schizophr Res. 2001;52:57-67. [ Links ]

21. Ford JM, Mathalon DH. Electrophysiological evidence of corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia during talking and thinking. J Psychiatr Res. 2004;38:37-46. [ Links ]

22. Wynn JK, Roach BJ, Lee J, Horan WP, Ford JM, Jimenez AM, et al. EEG findings of reduced neural synchronization during visual integration in schizophrenia. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0119849. [ Links ]

23. Levelt WJ. Lexical Access in Speech Production. Netherlands:Springer;2001. [ Links ]

24. Sauseng P, Klimesch W, Schabus M, Doppelmayr M. Fronto-parietal EEG coherence in theta and upper alpha reflect central executive functions of working memory. Int J Psychophysiol. 2005;57:97-103. [ Links ]

25. Wolf DH, Gur RC, Valdez JN, Loughead J, Elliott MA, Gur RE, et al. Alterations of fronto-temporal connectivity during word encoding in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2007;154:221-32. [ Links ]

26. van den Heuvel MP, Mandl RC, Stam CJ, Kahn RS, Pol HE. Aberrant frontal and temporal complex network structure in schizophrenia:a graph theoretical analysis. J Neurosci. 2010;30:15915-26. [ Links ]

27. Lin CL, Jung M, Wu YC, She HC, Jung TP. Neural correlates of mathematical problem solving. Int J Neural Syst. 2015;25:1550004. [ Links ]

28. Klimesch W. a-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16:606-17. [ Links ]

29. Doesburg SM, Vinette SA, Cheung MJ, Pang EW. Theta-modulated gamma-band synchronization among activated regions during a verb generation task. Front Psychol. 2012;3:195. [ Links ]

30. Inanaga K. Frontal midline theta rhythm and mental activity. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1998;52:555-66. [ Links ]

31. Zhang Y, Chen Y, Bressler SL, Ding M. Response preparation and inhibition:the role of the cortical sensorimotor beta rhythm. Neuroscience. 2008;156:238-46. [ Links ]

32. Marini A, Spoletini I, Rubino IA, Ciuffa M, Bria P, Martinotti G, et al. The language of schizophrenia:an analysis of micro and macrolinguistic abilities and their neuropsychological correlates. Schizophr Res. 2008;105:144-55. [ Links ]

FundingCONACYT 93034 to HJPG.

Ethical disclosures

Protection of human and animal subjects. The authors declare that the procedures followed were in accordance with the regulations of the relevant clinical research ethics committee and with those of the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki).

Confidentiality of data. The authors declare that they have followed the protocols of their work center on the publication of patient data.

Right to privacy and informed consent. The authors have obtained the written informed consent of the patients or subjects mentioned in the article. The corresponding author is in possession of this document.

Received: September 28, 2019; Accepted: November 30, 2019

*Correspondence: Héctor J. Pelayo-González Neuropsychology Unit Benemérita Universidad Autónoma de Puebla, Puebla E-mail: hector.pelayo@correo.buap.mx

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Creative Commons License Instituto Nacional de Cardiología Ignacio Chávez. Published by Permanyer. This is an open ccess article under the CC BY-NC-ND license