SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.29 número6La prueba de Rorschach y la personalidad antisocialEnmascaramiento. Un tipo de sincronización. Primera parte índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Revista

Articulo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • No hay artículos similaresSimilares en SciELO

Compartir


Salud mental

versión impresa ISSN 0185-3325

Salud Ment vol.29 no.6 México nov./dic. 2006

 

Artículos originales

Análisis del llanto en niños hipoacúsicos y normoyentes de 0 a 2 años de edad

Emilio Arch-Tirado1 

Antonio Verduzco-Mendoza1 

Mario Mandujano Valdés2 

Carlos Alberto Reyes-García3 

Alfonso Alfaro-Rodríguez1 

María del Carmen Sánchez2 

Carlos Fabián Martínez-Cruz4 

Verónica Taboada-Picazo1 

1Instituto Nacional de Rehabilitación.

2Laboratorio de Seguimiento del Neurodesarrollo. Universidad Autónoma Metropolitana-X /Instituto Nacional de Pediatría.

3Instituto Nacional de Astrofísica Optica y Electrónica-INAOE, México.

4 Instituto Nacional de Perinatología, México.


Resumen:

El llanto de los recién nacidos y lactantes es un fenómeno complejo que implica la producción de sonidos. Aunque no se sabe con certeza si el llanto constituye un sustrato para el lenguaje, los mecanismos fisiológicos de control del primero son similares a los que controlan el segundo. Así, el análisis del llanto provee información útil acerca del estado neurofisiológico y psicológico del neonato y del lactante, y permite la identificación de anormalidades del llanto infantil. Asimismo, puede proveer información útil relativa al desarrollo del lenguaje. Por último, el llanto se puede estudiar desde diversos enfoques para obtener información importante en cuanto a sus características fonológicas y acústicas. El objetivo del presente trabajo fue profundizar sobre el estudio del llanto del niño hipoacúsico. Para ello se exploraron las diferencias cuantitativas y las posibles diferencias cualitativas respecto de un grupo de neonatos sin patología, bajo el supuesto del control auditivo de las características del llanto.

Material y métodos.

Sujetos. Se estudiaron 20 niños de 0 a 2 años de edad, con hipoacusia profunda y 20 niños normoyentes. Se registró su llanto con una grabadora digital Sony durante la exploración física rutinaria. Los registros se analizaron en el Laboratorio de Bioacústica del Instituto Nacional de Rehabilitación, utilizando el software Cool Edit 96. El análisis se realizó comparando el grupo de hipoacúsicos con el de normoyentes. Para comparar los resultados entre grupos, se estimaron las medidas de tendencia central y dispersión y se contrastaron mediante análisis de varianza de una vía.

Resultados.

El promedio de la duración del llanto en el grupo de hipoacúsicos fue de 0.5845 ± 0.6150 seg, con un rango de 0.08 a 5.2 seg, en tanto que en el grupo control fue de 0.5387 ± 0.2631, con un rango de 0.06 a 1.75. En el grupo de los hipoacúsicos, se observaron cinco casos con una duración muy prolongada del llanto; en los análisis de varianza de una vía, las diferencias no alcanzaron significado estadístico. En el grupo control no se observaron casos con llanto prolongado. El promedio de los periodos inspiratorios fue de 0.3962 ± 0.2326, con un rango de 0.06 a 1.75, en los hipoacúsicos, y de 0.4083 ± 0.1854, con un rango de 0.21 a 0.96, en los controles. La exploración de datos mediante conglomerados se condujo con el método de Ward. La exploración de las tipologías con los grupos de hipoacúsicos y normoyentes no alcanzó significado estadístico.

En el análisis de contingencia, 57.49% de los llantos de los sordos fueron cortos, 40.72% de duración intermedia y 1.8% muy prolongados. La diferencia del promedio de los llantos pro longados con los de duración intermedia fue de 3.6 s, pero no alcanzó nivel significativo.

En el análisis cualitativo de los llantos se apreciaron cambios consistentes considerados anormales: vibrato, inestabilidad y deficiencia melódicas, consistentes en pérdida de la frecuencia fundamental(F0), pérdida o limitación en la producción de armónicos, llantos explosivos, deslizamientos, bifonación y pérdida de la intensidad del llanto al final del periodo. Aunque estos cambios pueden observarse en los controles, sólo llegan a presentarse de manera esporádica.

Análisis cualitativo de los espectrogramas. En primer término, se procedió a la estandarización del método. Dos observadores calibrados determinaron la presencia de características de variación de la F0 y de los armónicos, y se validó mediante el contraste gráfico con el análisis de Fourier en niños hipoacúsicos y normoyentes, todos ellos pacientes del Instituto Nacional de Rehabilitación.

Conclusiones

  1. El análisis cuantitativo del llanto de los hipoacúsicos no permite apreciar diferencias estadísticas significativas con respecto de los casos normoyentes.

  2. El análisis cualitativo de los espectrogramas del llanto de los hipoacúsicos es diferente de los normoyentes, por su deficiente control melódico y por la aparición de anormalidades.

  3. Se requiere profundizar en los estudios, especificando los detalles del método para mejorar la obtención y la comparabilidad de los datos.

  4. El espectrograma del llanto de los hipoacúsicos podrá apoyar a la terapéutica con el propósito de mejorar su calidad de fonación.

Palabras clave: Llanto; hipoacúsia; adquisición de lenguaje

Abstract:

Infant crying is a complex phenomenon that implies several functions: breathing, action of laryngeal and supralaryngeal muscles under the control of the neurovegetative systems of the brainstem, and the limbic system, and the association of cortical areas and the cerebellum. Although it is a communication system different to babbling and language, it is related with the future development of phonation. Cry analysis provides information about the neuro-physiologic and psychological states of newborns and the identification of perinatal abnormalities. It is necessary to discuss the subject extensively because there are new data on situations such as laringomalacia, congenital hypothyroidism, deafness and sleep apnea that seem to be associated to infantile crying behaviors.

Infant cries can be analyzed as behavioral conditions (hunger, anger and pain cries) allows knowing of mother-child relationship or the effect under diverse cultural conditions, such as stress, emo-tional deprivation or illness. A spectrographic analysis of the cries may identify several characteristics: threshold, latency, duration of phonation, maximum and minimum of the fundamental frequency (F0), occurrence and maximum pitch of shift, gliding, melody, biphonation, bifurcation, noise concentration, quality of the voice, double harmonic break, glottal plosives, vibratos, melody types, F0 stability and inspiratory stridor. To date, it has not been possible to establish alteration patterns. The best studied variables are F0, its harmonics and the duration of each emission; it is accepted that F0 varies between 400 and 600 Hz, during 1.4 ± 0.6s. Under such approaches, diverse alterations and risk factors have been studied: congenital alterations, malnutrition, sudden death, maternal exposition to drugs, prematurely born babies or perinatal asphyxia and disturbances of the central nervous system. Authors have reported F0 equal or less than 300 Hz in cases of sudden death or with high frequencies, near the 1000 Hz in the Cri du chat syndrome, perinatal asphyxia and other cases who died suddenly. During the cry, there is an increase of intra-abdominal pressure, heart rate and blood pressure, reduction of oxygen saturation, increase of the intra cranial-pressure, beginning of stress reactions, depletion of the energy anf oxygen reserves, such as the found in the Valsalva's maneuver. Every event of prolonged cries implies alteration of the breathing control like a Hering-Breuer reflex. Considering that some authors have proposed early vocalizations are a good predictor of deafness, in a previous paper we reported the characteristics of the cry of 20 deaf neonates. However, we were not able to demonstrate differences when comparing them with normal hearing neonates and infants, using only parametric methods. Still, we decided to go further and investgate the quality of infant cries of deaf neonates and infants.

Material and methods.

Twenty zero-to two-year old cases were studied; they were deaf children of both sexes; all cases were included in a follow-up program on the Human Communication Department of the National Institute of Perinatology of Mexico and were compared with 20 normal hearing children. We re-corded Brain Stem Evoked Auditory Responses (BEAR) and cry recording using a digital Sony recorder during the physical exploration. We analyzed the frequency (Hz) and duration of the espiratory cries, the duration of inspiration between two cry emissions and the characteristics of the spectrogram.

Quantitative analysis. The usual estimates of means and standard variation were obtained and they were compared with one way analysis of variance. We organized typologies of frequency by means of cluster techniques (Ward method). The distribution of the duration of the periods of crying and silence was explored with a contingency tables.

Qualitative analysis. Two standardized observers visually analyzed all the cries to determine any variation of F0 and of harmonic frequencies. Whenever a variation of F0 was observed, we obtained maximum and minimum frequencies, as well as average duration of each cry emission. The procedure was validated by means of the graphic comparison with a Fouries analysis.

Results.

Mean duration of cries in the deaf group was 0.5845 ± 0.6150 s (range 0.08-5.2 s), while in the group of normal hearing cases was 0.5387 ± 0.2631 (range 0.06-1.75 s). From the deaf group, five cases had very prolonged duration of cries, without statistical significance. The mean duration of the inspiration was 0.3962 ± 0.2326, with a range of 0.06 to 1.75 in the deaf group and of 0.4083 ± 0.1854, with a range of 0.21 at 0.96, in the controls, without difference among groups. There was no correlation between the time of espiratory cry and that of the inspiration. Three cry topologies were organized: one of shorter duration (mean 0.30 s), with 111 spectrograms, an intermediate one (mean 0.73) with 85 spectrograms and one of prolonged duration (mean 4.5 s) with spectrograms of three cases. Three topologies of the inspiratory period were obtained: one of short periods (mean 0.33 s), with 171 spectrograms, one of intermediate duration (mean 0.80 s) with 18 spectrograms and one of prolonged duration (mean 1.60 s) with three cases. There were no statistical differences of tipologies between the deaf groups and normal hearing cases. On the qualitative analysis of cries, we came across several variations which are interpreted as abnormalities: vibratos, poor melodic control, loss of fundamental frequencies, harmonic limited production, plosives, gliding, bi phonation, and a loss of intensity at end of cry emissions. These changes were also observed on the control cases, but only in a very limited number.

Discussion.

Cry spectrogram analysis are non invasive indicators of the neonate's neurophysiologic organization. Although cry duration varies in healthy newborns, the accepted variation for a normal range is 1.1 to 2.8 s, with standard deviations around 0.6 s. Consistent differences have not been demonstrated between risk and control groups. However, abnormal cases such as Down syndrome or severe asphyxia have very short cries, whereas on the Cri du chat syndrome the duration of cries is prolonged. Extended cries imply cardiac and respiratory risks which have been associated with later outcomes as development retardation and sudden death. There are also some questions to solve, such as the regulation and control of cry, starting from breathing mechanisms or from a sensorial afferent, mediated by hearing. The deaf infants are constituted in a study model, considering that the auditory afference is suppressed and the control of the cry is restricted to the breathing environment. In the studied spectrograms, the duration of the cry was within reported normal limits by other authors, inasmuch in the normal hearing control cases as in the deaf, except the dissident cases, but without these reaching statistical significance. Further research of brainstem function is needed for the abnormal cases with prolonged cry periods, since such cries are interpreted as an alteration of the breathing reflexes of Hering-Breuer, which might have a pathological meaning in the sense of the sob's spasm or even more severe risk factors as sleep apnea and even sudden death.

The qualitative analysis in the deaf individuals demonstrated a poor quality and unstable character of melodic control, with a smaller number of harmonics. The deaf cases lost the relationship between the fundamental frequencies and their harmonics, mainly because of the participation of supraglottic structures that modulate pitch and due to the poorness of melodic control, either for monotony or due to the impossibility of returning to a normal pattern, following variations such as vibrato, plosives or noise concentration. In the cases of prolonged cries, starting from the third second, the sound intensity tends to diminish and the harmonics are lost, perhaps due to a decrease of the subglottal pressure of phonation. This finding supports the auditory control of crying related to breathing mechanisms.

Conclusions.

In preliminary terms, by means of the melodic analysis of the spectrograms, differences are demonstrated be-tween the cries of the deaf and of the normal hearing cases. The increase of the complexity of the melody of the cry, or their poverty, are indicative of the neuromuscular function and they may support the evaluation of phonation before language development. The study of the spectrograms of deaf individuals does not constitute an element for the detection or for diagnosis since, to date, estimators of sensibility or of specificity have not been established, but they constitute a support for its integral evaluation, with the possibility of evaluating and of improving therapeutic rehabilitation.

Key words: Cry; deafness; language adquisition

Texto disponible solo en PDF

Referencias

1. Arch-Tirado E, Mandujano M, Garcia-Torices L, Martinez-Cruz CF, Reyes-Garcia CA, Taboada-Picazo V: Análisis del llanto del niño hipoacúsico y del niño normo oyente. Cirugía Cirujanos, 72(4):271-276, 2004. [ Links ]

2. Bell RQ: A congenital contribution to emotional response in early infancy and the preschool period. Ciba Found Symp, (33):201-12, 1975. [ Links ]

3. Christensson K, Cabrera T, Christensson E, Uvnas-Moberg K, Winberg J: Separation distress cali in the human neonate in the absence of maternal body contact. Acta Paediatr, 84(5):468-73 1995. [ Links ]

4. Colton RH, Steinschneider A: The cry characteristics of an infant who died of the sudden infant death syndrome. J Speech Hear Disord, 46(4):359-63, 1981. [ Links ]

5. Corwin MJ, Lester BM, Sepkoski C, Mclaughlin S, Kayne H, Golub HL: Effects of in utero cocaine exposure on newborn acoustical cry characteristics. Pediatrics, 89(6 Pt 2):1199-203, 1992. [ Links ]

6. Corwin MJ, Lester BM, Sepkoski C, Peucker M, Kayne H, Golub HL: Newborn acoustic cry characteristics of infants subsequently dying of sudden infant death syndrome. Pediatrics, 96(1 Pt 1):73-7, 1995. [ Links ]

7. Eilers R, Oller DK: Infant vocalizations and the early diagnosis of severe hearing impairment. J Pediatr Psychol, 124(2):199-203, 1994. [ Links ]

8. Fort A, Ismaelli A, Manfredi C, Bruscaglioni P: Parametric and non-parametric estimation of speech formants: application to infant cry. Med Eng Phys, 18(8):677-91, 1996. [ Links ]

9. Fort A, Manfredi C: Acoustic analysis of newborn infant cry signals. Med Eng Phys, 20(6):432-42, 1998. [ Links ]

10. Garbaruk V: The acoustic characteristics of the vocalizations of hypoacusic children. Vestn Otorinolaringol, 4:62-4, 1998. [ Links ]

11. Goberman AM, Robb MP: Acoustic examination of preterm and full-term infant cries: the long-time average spectrum. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 42(4):850-61, 1999. [ Links ]

12. Lester BM, Anderson LT, Boukydis CF, Garcia-Coll CT, Vohr B, Peucker M: Early detection of infants at risk for later handicap through acoustic cry analysis. Birth Defects Orig Artic Ser, 25(6):99-118, 1989. [ Links ]

13. Lester BM, Boukydis CF, Lagasse L: Cardiorespiratory reactivity during the Brazelton Scale in term and preterm infants. J Pediatr Psychol, 21(6):771-83, 1996. [ Links ]

14. Lester BM, Corwin MJ, Sepkoski C, Seifer R, Peucker M, Mclaughlin S y cols.: Neurobehavioral syndromes in cocaine-exposed newborn infants. Child Dev, 62(4):694-705, 1991. [ Links ]

15. Lester BM: Developmental outcome prediction from acoustic cry analysis in term and preterm infants. Pediatrics, 80(4):529-34, 1987. [ Links ]

16. Lester BM. Spectrum analysis of the cry sounds of well-nourished and malnourished infants. Child Dev, 47(1):237-41, 1976. [ Links ]

17. Ludington-Hoe SM, Cong X, Hashemi F: Infant crying: nature, physiologic consequences, and select interventions. Neonatal Netw, 21(2):29-36, 2002. [ Links ]

18. Metz D, Whitehead R, Whitehead B: Mechanics of vocal fold vibration and laryngeal articulatory gestures produced by hearing-impaired speakers. J Speech Hear Res, 27(1):62-69, 1984. [ Links ]

19. Michelsson K, Eklund K, Leppanen P, Lyytinen H: Cry characteristics of 172 healthy 1-to 7-day-old infants. Folia Phoniatr Logop, 54(4):190-200, 2002. [ Links ]

20. Michelsson K, Michelsson O: Phonation in the newborn, infant cry. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol, 49 (Supl 1):S297-301, 1999. [ Links ]

21. Michelsson K, Sirvo P, Koivisto M, Sovijarvi A, Wasz-HOckert O: Spectrographic analysis of pain cry in neonates with cleft palate. Biol Neonate, 26(5-6):353-8, 1975. [ Links ]

22. Michelsson K, Sirvio P, Wasz-Hockert O: Sound spectrographic cry analysis of infants with bacterial meningitis. Dev Med Child Neurol, 19(3):309-15, 1977. [ Links ]

23. Móller S, Schónweiler R: Analysis of infant cries for the early detection of hearing impairment. Speech Commun, 175-193, 1999. [ Links ]

24. Parvizi J, Anderson SW, Martin CO, Damasio H, Damasio AR: Pathological laughter and crying: a link to the cerebellum. Brain, 124(Pt 9):1708-19, 2001. [ Links ]

25. Robb MP, Cacace AT: Estimation of formant frequencies in infant cry. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol, 32(1):57-67, 1995. [ Links ]

26. Robb MP, Goberman AM, Cacace AT: An acoustic template of newborn infant crying. Folia Phoniatr Logop, 49(1):35-41, 1997. [ Links ]

27. Rothgangerh Ueberschar, R: The infant cry and its diagnostic significance. Z Arztl Fortbild, 74(11):537-40, 1980. [ Links ]

28. Scheiner E HK, Jurgens U, Zwirner P: The influence of hearing impairment on preverbal emotional vocalizations of infants. Folia Phoniatr Logop, 56(1):27-40, 2004. [ Links ]

29. Schonweiler R, Kaese S, Moller S, Rinscheid A, Ptok M: Use of self-organizing neural networks (Kohonen maps) for classification of voice acoustic signals exemplified by the infant voice with and without time-delayed auditory feedback. Hno, 44(4):201-6, 1996. [ Links ]

30. SchonweileR R, Kaese S, Moller S, Rinscheid A, Ptok M: Neuronal networks and self-organizing maps: new computer techniques in the acoustic evaluation of the infant cry. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol, 38(1):1-11, 1996. [ Links ]

31. Wasz-Hockert O, Michelsson K, Lind J: Twenty-five Years of Scandinavian Cry Research. En: Lester BM, Boukydis CF (eds.). Infant Crying. Theoretical and Reserarch Perspectives. Plenum Press, Nueva York, 1985. [ Links ]

32. Wermke K, Hauser C, Komposch G, Stellzig A: Spectral analysis of prespeech sounds (spontaneous cries) in infants with unilateral cleft lip and palate (UCLP): a pilot study. Cleft Palate Craniofac J, 39(3):285-94, 2002. [ Links ]

33. Wermke K, Mende W, Manfredi C, Bruscaglioni P: Developmental aspects of infant's cry melody and formants. Med Eng Phys, 24(7-8):501-14, 2002. [ Links ]

34. Zeskind PS, Lester BM: Analysis of cry features in newborns with differential fetal growth. Child Dev, 52(1):207-12, 1981. [ Links ]

35. Zeskind PS, Marshall TR, Goff DM: Cry threshold predicts regulatory disorder in newborn infants. J Pediatr Psychol, 21(6):803-19, 1996. [ Links ]

Recibido: 27 de Junio de 2006; Aprobado: 20 de Julio de 2006

Correspondencia: Dr. Emilio Arch Tirado. Instituto Nacional de Rehabilitación. Torre de Investigación 2do piso. Laboratorio de Bioacústica. Calz. México-Xochimilco 289, col. Arenal de Guadalupe, Tlalpan, 14389 México DF. arch@infosel.net.mx Tel.59991000 ext. 19312.

Creative Commons License Este es un artículo publicado en acceso abierto bajo una licencia Creative Commons