SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.29 número3Agorafobia (con o sin pánico) y conductas de afrontamiento desadaptativas. Estudio empírico. Segunda parteUbicación de la alexitimia en relación a los rasgos psicopatológicos de personalidad del MMPI índice de autoresíndice de assuntospesquisa de artigos
Home Pagelista alfabética de periódicos  

Serviços Personalizados

Journal

Artigo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • Não possue artigos similaresSimilares em SciELO

Compartilhar


Salud mental

versão impressa ISSN 0185-3325

Salud Ment vol.29 no.3 México Mai./Jun. 2006

 

Artículos originales

Influencia de la cantidad y la calidad subjetiva de sueño en diversas características de personalidad

Elena Miró1 

Pilar Martínez1 

Raimundo Arriaza1 

1Departamento de Personalidad, Evaluación y Tratamientos Psicológicos, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Granada, Campus Universitario de la Cartuja s/n., CP. 18071, Granada (España). E-mail: emiro@ugr.es


Resumen:

Según las diferencias individuales en la duración habitual del sueño, suele establecerse una distinción entre los sujetos con un patrón de sueño corto (seis horas de sueño o menos al día), los sujetos con un patrón de sueño intermedio (entre siete y ocho horas al día) y aquellos con un patrón de sueño largo (más de nueve horas al día). Aunque no se sabe a qué se deben tales diferencias, en los últimos años se ha ido demostrando que dormir menos tiempo y, paradójicamente también, más tiempo del asociado con el patrón de sueño intermedio tiene un impacto negativo en la salud física.

Hay muy pocos estudios sobre las posibles diferencias psicológicas entre los patrones de sueño. A excepción de los conocidos trabajos de Hartmann o Hicks de la década de 1970, un aspecto que apenas se ha analizado es si los patrones de sueño difieren según las características de la personalidad. Las dimensiones de la personalidad que han recibido más atención han sido la extraversión y el neuroticismo, sin que en la mayoría de los estudios se observen diferencias en función del patrón de sueño. No obstante, en el caso del neuroticismo, los resultados son contradictorios al existir también informes que sí establecen dichas diferencias. No haber considerado, junto con la cantidad de sueño otros aspectos esenciales del sueño, como la calidad del mismo, quizá pueda explicar parte de la inconsistencia de los hallazgos encontrados en la bibliografía. Por otra parte, falta investigación sobre factores como el psicoticismo o los límites cognitivos que, no obstante, se ha demostrado que guardan relaciones interesantes con otros aspectos del sueño.

El presente trabajo analiza la influencia del patrón de sueño (corto, intermedio y largo), la calidad de sueño (alta, media y baja) y la posible interacción entre ambos factores con el neuroticismo, el psicoticismo y los límites cognitivos.

La muestra estuvo integrada por 125 estudiantes sanos (110 mujeres y 15 varones), de 18 a 26 años, seleccionados mediante un cuestionario de sueño elaborado para este propósito. Este exploraba los hábitos de sueño de los sujetos y controlaba que no presentaran problemas médicos, psicológicos ni estuvieran to mando fármacos. Los participantes del estudio se distribuyeron del siguiente modo: 1. Sujetos con patrón de sueño corto (n=20), 2. Sujetos con patrón de sueño intermedio (n=82), y 3. Sujetos con patrón de sueño largo (n=23). A su vez, dentro de cada uno de los patrones de sueño, se establecieron tres subgrupos si la calidad subjetiva de sueño era alta, media o baja.

El neuroticismo y el psicoticismo se evaluaron con el Cuestionario de Personalidad de Eysenck (EPQ). Los sujetos también llenaron los Inventarios de Ansiedad y Depresión de Beck (BAI y BDI, respectivamente), que ya se habían considerado en otra publicación. Se excluyó a los sujetos con puntuaciones superiores a 18 en el BAI y BDI, o superiores al centil 70 en el EPQ, a fin de intentar garantizar que la muestra no presentara alteraciones psicológicas.

Se efectuaron ANOVAs para examinar si el patrón de sueño, la calidad subjetiva del mismo o la interacción entre ambos factores ejercían un efecto significativo en cualquiera de las variables analizadas. Los contrastes post hoc se realizaron con las pruebas de Scheffé o Tamhane. Los resultados mostraron que los sujetos con peor calidad de sueño puntuaban más alto en neuroticismo (15.14) que los que tenían una calidad de sueño media (13.23) o buena (9.96). Por el contrario, la dimensión de psicoticismo estaba influida por la cantidad de sueño pero no por la calidad del mismo. Los sujetos con patrón de sueño largo puntuaban ligeramente más alto en psicoticismo (2.57) que los sujetos con patrón de sueño intermedio (1.52) o corto (1.25). La dimensión de límites cognitivos no se relacionó con ninguno de estos aspectos. En ninguna de las variables analizadas existió una interacción significativa entre cantidad y calidad de sueño.

Se ofrecen algunas hipótesis sobre los mecanismos que pueden intervenir en las relaciones observadas. Se destaca también que es importante tomar en serio las consecuencias asociadas a los modelos que se apartan de la duración y la calidad óptimas de sueño. Para ello es necesario fomentar iniciativas preventivas y educativas destinadas a mejorar nuestros hábitos de sueño.

Palabras clave: Duración habitual del sueño; calidad subjetiva de sueño; neuroticismo; psicoticismo; límites cognitivos

Abstract:

In relation to individual differences in the habitual duration of sleep, a distinction can be established between subjects having a short sleep pattern (6 hours or less of sleep per night), subjects with an intermediate sleep pattern (between 7 and 8 hours of sleep), and subjects with a long sleep pattern (more than 9 hours of sleep). The reason for these individual differences in sleep duration is unknown.

Diverse studies have been carried out in an attempt to understand if psychological or physiological differences exist in people with distinct sleep patterns. Recently, it has been demonstrated that sleeping less and, paradoxically, sleeping more than the sleep quantity associated with the intermediate sleep pattern (7-8 hours) has a negative impact on physical health.

On the contrary, studies about possible psychological differences between different sleep patterns are almost nonexistent. Some studies that analyze variables regarding vigilance suggest that subjects with a long sleep pattern have a poorer performance in tests of vigilance than subjects who have a short sleep pattern. In turn, subjects with short sleep pattern appear to have more academic efficiency problems and appear to show a more depressed mood state than the subjects belonging to the other sleep pattern groups.

One aspect that has been scarcely analyzed, with the exception of the classic works by Hartmann and Hicks in the 1970's, is if sleep patterns differ according to personality characteristics. The dimensions of personality which have received more attention have been extraversion and neuroticism, and none of the majority of studies has observed any significative differences in function of sleep patterns. Nevertheless, in the case of neuroticism, results are contradictory and there exist also reports that observe differences in sleep pattern function. It is important to emphasize that in these studies the quantity of sleep was not considered along with other essential aspects of sleep such as quality. Perhaps this aspect could explain a part of the inconsistent findings in the literature.

On the other hand, psychoticism, which along with extraversion and neuroticism constitutes the third big dimension of the known tripartite model of personality, has been the least investigated personality dimension. Again, no work exists which analyzes the relations between the pattern of sleep and the dimension of personality of cognitive limits more recently proposed by Hartmann.

The present study is a part of a wider investigation, the objective of which is to analyze the relations between the subjective quantity and quality of sleep and psychological variables in healthy individuals. This paper is centered on the influence of the pattern of sleep (short, intermediate, and long), the subjective quality of sleep (high, medium, or low) and the possible interaction between both factors in the personality dimensions of neuroticism, psychoticism, and cognitive limits.

The sample was composed of 125 healthy students (110 women and 15 males) with ages ranging from 18 to 26 years old. The participants were selected according to their responses to a sleep questionnaire created for this purpose, which explored the habits of sleep, the state of medical health, past and present psychological condition, and possible use of medication. All the subjects selected showed good medical and psychological health, they did not use any type of medication, nor did they belong to any extreme morning or evening type of circadian rhythm. Each subject had a regular bedtime hour between 11:30 p.m. and 2:30 a.m. and waking hour between 7:30 a.m. and 10:30 a.m.

The subjects selected were divided into three groups in accordance with the number of hours they habitually slept in order to feel good during the day: 1. subjects with short sleep pattern (n=20), 2. subjects with intermediate sleep pattern (n=82) and finally, 3. subjects with long sleep pattern (n=23).

Additionally, other three groups were established within each of the sleep patterns considering if the quality of sleep was high, medium, or low. In short sleep pattern group the quality of sleep reported as high, medium, and low was 25%, 40%, and 35%, respectively. These percentages were 42.68%, 43.9%, and 13.41% in the group with an intermediate sleep pattern; and 30.43%, 52.17%, and 17.39% in the group with a long sleep pattern.

The personality dimensions of neuroticism and psychoticism were evaluated with The Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ-A). The cognitive boundaries were evaluated with The Boundary Questionnaire (BQ). In addition, subjects completed the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) and the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) (which have not been taken into consideration here). The criteria of exclusion were a score higher than 18 on the BDI or the BAI and a score higher than 70 on the dimensions of neuroticism and psychoticism. These last exclusions were established to make assure the subjects were free of psychological dysfunction.

Two-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were performed to assess the effects of the quantity of sleep (short, intermediate, or long sleep pattern), and the subjective quality of sleep (high, medium, or low) and the possible interaction between both factors in the case of each variable. The Levene test was used to examine variance homogeneity. Likewise, the Scheffé Test (for equal variances) and the Tamhane Test (for unequal variances) were used as post hoc contrast statistics.

The results showed the dimension of neuroticism was influenced by subjective sleep quality, but not by habitual sleep duration. Subjects with a poor subjective sleep quality scored higher on neuroticism (15.14) than those who had a medium (13.23) or good (9,96) sleep quality. Contrarily, the personality dimension of psychoticism was influenced by sleep quantity, but not quality. Subjects with a low sleep pattern scored slightly higher in psychoticism (2.57) than those with intermediate (1.52) or short (1.25) sleep patterns. The dimension of cognitive boundaries was not related with any of these aspects. There was not any significant interaction between sleep quantity and sleep quality for the analyzed variables. This result highlights the need to evaluate sleep quantity as well as sleep quality, treating them as two relatively independent measures that provide complementary information.

The highest scores of neuroticism of the group with the worse quality of sleep are consistent with recent reports showing that being worried or anxious disturbs the normal appearance of slow wave sleep (phases 3 and 4). The expression of this sleep phase is psychologically linked with sleep quality. On the other hand, it may be the case that the highest scores in psychoticism obtained by the subjects with long sleep pattern relate with the extra quantity of REM they obtain by sleeping a greater number of hours. This phase of sleep has been associated with mood regulation and psychological balance. In polysomnographic studies, subjects with long sleep pattern are characterized as having a greater quantity of phase 1, 2, and REM sleep and less quantities of slow wave sleep than the other sleep patterns.

However, the present data are correlational and not casual. Thus, the mechanisms which could be influencing in the observed relationships are unknown. Similarly, it is unclear how sleep pattern differences might translate into psychological or biological changes which may affect personality, mood, or health. Future longitudinal research, including objective sleep measurements in healthy subjects, as well as in subjects with sleep disorders of different degrees, may contribute to the clarification of these mechanisms. In any case, sleep seems to be an excellent indicator of several psychological characteristics and so the consequences associated with models which deviate from the intermediate sleep pattern deserve to be taken seriously. It is also important to develop preventive and educational initiatives to optimize our sleeping habits.

Key words: Habitual sleep duration; subjective sleep quality; neuroticism; psychoticism; cognitive boundaries

Texto disponible solo en PDF

Referencias

1. Aeschbach D, Cajochen C, Landolt H, Borbely AA: Homeostatic sleep regulation in habitual short sleepers and long sleepers. Am J Physiol, 270:41-53, 1996. [ Links ]

2. Ayas NT, White DP, Manson JE, Stampfer MJ, Speizer FE y cols.: A prospective study of sleep duration and coronary heart disease in women. Arch Intern Med, 163:205-209, 2003. [ Links ]

3. Buela-Casal G, Miro E: ¿Qué es el Sueño? Para qué Dormimos y para qué Soñamos. Biblioteca Nueva. Madrid, 2001. [ Links ]

4. Buela-Casal G, Sierra JC, Caballo VE: Personality differences between short and long sleepers. Pers Indiv Differ,13:115-117, 1992. [ Links ]

5. Cano MC, Miro E, Espinosa-Fernandez L, Buela-Casal G: Parámetros subjetivos de sueño y estado de ánimo disfórico. Rev Psicopatol Psicol Clín, 27:120-137, 2004. [ Links ]

6. Cartwright R, Young MA, Mercer P, Bears M: Role of REM sleep and dream variables in prediction of remission from depression. Psychiat Res, 80:249-255, 1998. [ Links ]

7. Chattopadhyay PK, Dasgupta SK: Trait anxiety neuroticism and extraversión in long and short sleeper children. Bangladesh J Psychol, 13:1-5, 1992. [ Links ]

8. Cowen D, Levin R: The use of the Hartmann Boundary Questionnaire with an adolescent population. Dreaming, 5:105-114, 1995. [ Links ]

9. Doi Y, Minova M, Tango T: Impact and correlates of poor sleep quality in Japanese white-collar employees. Sleep, 26:467-471, 2003. [ Links ]

10. Eysenck HJ, Eysenck SBG: Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (Junior & Adult). Hodder and Stoughton. Londres, 1975. [ Links ]

11. Golden A, Twery M, Hunt CE: National Center on Sleep Disorders Research. Sleep Med, 5:83-85, 2004. [ Links ]

12. Gray EK, Watson D: General and specific traits of personality and their relation to sleep and academic performance. J Pers, 70:177-206, 2002. [ Links ]

13. Harrison Y, Horne JA: Should we be taking more sleep?. Sleep, 18:901-907, 1995. [ Links ]

14. Hartmann E: Sleep requeriment: Long sleepers, short sleepers, variable sleepers and insomniacs. Psychosomatics, 14: 95-103, 1973. [ Links ]

15. Hartmann E: Boundaries of the Mind. Basic Books. Nueva York, 1991. [ Links ]

16. Hicks RA: Normal insomnia: Its beneficts and its costs. Annual Scholar's Address. San José State University, 1983. [ Links ]

17. Hicksr A, Marical CM, Konti PA: Coping with a major stressor: differences between habitual short and longer sleepers. Percept Motor Skill, 72:631-636, 1991. [ Links ]

18. Iwawaki S, Sarmany-Schuller I: Cross-cultural (Japan-Slovakia) comparison of some aspects of sleeping patterns and anxiety. Stud Psychol, 43:215-224, 2001. [ Links ]

19. Kecklund G, Akerstedt T: Apprehension of the subsequent working day is associated with a low amount of slow wave sleep. Biol Psychol, 66:169-178, 2004. [ Links ]

20. Kelly W, Kelly K, Clanton RC : The relationship between sleep length and grade-point average among college students. Coll Stud J, 35:84-86, 2001. [ Links ]

21. Kripke DF, Garfinkel L, Wingard D, Klauber MR, Marter MR: Mortality associated with sleep duration and insomnia. Arch Gen Psychiat, 59:131-136, 2002. [ Links ]

22. Kumar A, Vaidya AK: Neuroticism in short and long sleepers. Percept Motor Skill, 54:962, 1982. [ Links ]

23. Mckelvie SJ: Sleep duration and self-reported Type A behavior: A replication. J Psychol, 126:285-289, 1992. [ Links ]

24. Miro E, Iañez MA, Cano-Lozano MC: Patrones de sueño y salud. Int Rev Clin Psicol Health, 2:301-326, 2002. [ Links ]

25. Miro E, Iañez MA, Catena A, Buela-Casal G: Calidad de sueño en sujetos con diferentes patrones habituales de sueño. Psicología y Salud, 13:193-202, 2003. [ Links ]

26. Miro E, Martínez Narváez P: Affective and personality characteristics in function of nightmare prevalence, nightmare distress and interference due to nightmares. Dreaming, 15:89-106, 2005. [ Links ]

27. Miro E, Martínez Narváez P, Arriaza R: Influencia de la cantidad y la calidad subjetiva de sueño en la ansiedad y el estado de ánimo deprimido. Salud Mental, 29(2):30-37, 2006. [ Links ]

28. Monk TH, Buysse DJ, Welsh DK, Kennedy KS, Rose LR: A sleep diary and questionnaire study of naturally short sleepers. J Sleep Res, 10:173-179, 2001. [ Links ]

29. Ohayon MM,Vecchierini MF: Normative sleep data, cognitive function and daily living activities in older adults in the Community. Sleep, 28:981-989, 2005. [ Links ]

30. Peters BR, Joireman J, Ridgway RL: Individual differences in the consideration of future consequences scale correlate with sleep habits, sleep quality, and GPA in university students. Psychol Rep: 96:817-824, 2005. [ Links ]

31. Schredl M: Effects of state and traits factors on nightmare frequency. Eur Arch Psychiat Clin Neurosci, 253: 241-247, 2003. [ Links ]

32. Vera-Villarroel PE, Sánchez AI, Rivera I, Buela-Casal G: Sleep pattern and daytime differences in the electromyographic activity and peripheral temperature. Int J Psychophysiol, 41:181-186, 2000. [ Links ]

33. Wagner MK, Mooney DK: Personality characteristics of long and short sleepers. J Clin Psychol, 31:434-436, 1975. [ Links ]

Recibido: 16 de Agosto de 2004; Aprobado: 16 de Febrero de 2006

Creative Commons License Este es un artículo publicado en acceso abierto bajo una licencia Creative Commons