SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.23 número3El legado del polio: 2 casos de síndrome postpolio y revisiónTrombectomía mecánica en niños: un recurso poco conocido y raramente utilizado índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Revista

Articulo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • No hay artículos similaresSimilares en SciELO

Compartir


Revista mexicana de neurociencia

versión On-line ISSN 2604-6180versión impresa ISSN 1665-5044

Rev. mex. neurocienc. vol.23 no.3 Ciudad de México may./jun. 2022  Epub 06-Jun-2022

https://doi.org/10.24875/rmn.21000029 

Review articles

Systematic review and effect of conscious breathing on adult attention and learning task

Revisión sistemática, efecto de la respiración consciente en la atención de los adultos y tarea de aprendizaje

Viviana Chavez1 

Lenin Ochoa-de-la-Paz2 

Jorge Parodi1  * 

1Laboratorio Biosocial Tonalli ltda, Tiburcio Saavedra, Temuco, Chile

2Department of Biochemistry, Faculty of Medicine, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Mexico City, Mexico


Abstract

In recent years, the control of physiological processes has been strengthened, which show metabolic changes that can generate effects at a neuronal level. Our objective is to review what has been described as conscious breathing theory and how this can affect cognitive tasks, which have been described as very affected in adults and which reduce with age. However, we have begun to understand a new phenomenon of andragogic learning, which indicates that the task of learning must be understood differently, and that attention is something that must be understood as something that does not decrease with age but changes. It also seems that conscious breathing tasks can improve this type of task and that it can be beneficial. What has been published suggests that breathing, or at least conscious breathing exercise, improves certain cellular and neuronal capacities and that this can lead to real changes in cognitive tasks in adults.

Keywords Meditation; Learning; Breathing; Aging

Resumen

En los últimos años se ha potenciado el control de los procesos fisiológicos, que muestran cambios metabólicos que pueden generar efectos a nivel neuronal. Nuestro objetivo es revisar lo que se ha descrito como teoría de la respiración consciente y cómo ésta puede afectar a las tareas cognitivas, que se han descrito como muy afectadas en los adultos y que se reducen con la edad. Sin embargo, hemos empezado a comprender un nuevo fenómeno de aprendizaje andragógico, que indica que la tarea de aprendizaje debe entenderse de forma diferente, y que la atención es algo que debe entenderse como algo que no disminuye con la edad, sino que cambia. También parece que las tareas de respiración consciente pueden mejorar este tipo de tareas y que pueden ser beneficiosas. Lo que se ha publicado sugiere que la respiración, o al menos el ejercicio de respiración consciente, mejora ciertas capacidades celulares y neuronales y que esto puede llevar a cambios reales en las tareas cognitivas en los adultos.

Palabras clave Meditación; Aprendizaje; Respiración; Envejecimiento

General ideas

For several years, attempts have been made to reconcile meditation tasks and cognitive function. However, it has been difficult to reconcile esoteric definitions with the real scientific background; we can observe in this type of exercise1. Luckily, and driven by various researchers, more pragmatic study methods have recently been applied to this type of activity and we can indicate a relation between physiological balance states, secondary to conscious breathing activities, and beneficial effects with cognitive tasks2. This review aims to present psychological, physiological, and neurophysiological evidence of the advantage of conscious breathing practice on cognitive tasks, particularly learning in the adults.

The task of learning and the andragogy

Defining this activity is complex, with various viewpoints according to the professional area which studies it. These are all variables to respect, but I must agree on one to define it. A decision, we would like to construct, is that learning will be any stimulus generating a behavioral change which leads to survival3. These ideas have also been reinforced by the introduction of the concept of synaptic plasticity4 and have been reviewed as an important and fundamental phenomenon for the task of learning5. Thus, this neurophysiological phenomenon will be a focus for observing adults when we consider the task of conscious breathing.

The history of definitions of education is long. Within it, pedagogy or teaching has been installed as education for children perpetuated in various models6. However, the concept of andragogy arose with strength in the 1960s, as a new identity for who was to be taught7. This model was taken as teaching adults, but neurobiological concepts let us redefine it as the process of educating a mature, full, and conscious brain, something which arises once this organ is already mature, and the ego emerges8. Learning in adults, andragogy recognizes the importance of tutor participation as an extrinsic motivator7, which concurs with support in guided information searches9. It defines learning in context, which suggests that continuous learning exists, where in any moment; we can acquire new skills10.

Attention as a complex task, description on adults

Within the task of learning, attention is a phenomenon to be studied11 as we can define this concept in neurosciences. We can use the concept that attention is a form of feedback, of self-stimulating learning processes12. Thus, we can think of it as a part of learning and deem it important to develop to achieve better learning task performance13.

Attention is a fundamental axis of the teaching-learning process, and in this way, we will consider it to be selective14. Furthermore, implicit learning modulates the assignation of attention in the first stages of the perceptive processing of learning and will become implicit only when attending to the relevant predictive information15. The information is redundant because its subjects can, in principle, select the right answer based on only one of these signs and ignore the others, so that consequently this signal is learned and the others are not14. This could basically be solved in adult individuals and is achieved in the selection of tasks which are known and to a lesser degree with new tasks. Subjects can also learn about stimuli which do not reach consciousness, particularly when they are near the perception threshold and are combined with a reward16, a topic which becomes relevant in earlier stages of life. Attention, environment, and reward jointly determine how we learn and are some of the factors determining neuronal plasticity17, which makes it even more important to investigate new learning environments and their determining factors, such as attentive breathing.

Imaging changes in learning tasks

There is apparently an idea that learning new tasks become harder with aging, especially those tasks depending on the hippocampus and structures related to the medial temporal lobe to form episodic or declarative memories. It has even been suggested that age-related cognitive deficits begin at age 2018. Various studies suggest that not only are there changes in cerebral microstructure and volume manifesting in cognitive function shifts19 but also metabolic changes20. Decreased cerebral mass and diminished cognitive function are often seen with advanced age20. Therefore, it is important to distinguish between metabolic changes in the brain caused by healthy aging and pathological states21.

Conscious breathing, physiology and molecular changes

The term meditation covers a wide range of mental training practices varying between cultures and traditions. They go from techniques for promoting physical health, relaxation, or improved concentration, developing an improved sensation of well-being and cultivating altruistic behavior22. Meditation is generally associated with a concurrent state of greater waking consciousness and lower metabolic activity, leading to better physical health, psychological balance, and emotional stability23. Mindfulness meditation is a promising basis for interventions, with particular potential relevance for psychiatric comorbidity. The successes and challenges of researching mindfulness meditation are based on the interactions between contemplative traditions and clinical psychological science24. The conclusions of these studies determined that attention is the fundamental process strengthened in these interventions25. To understand this, we can investigate that evidence exists supporting mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for treating depression26 and obsessive-compulsive disorder. At present, there are studies about its efficacy on chronic and terminal diseases. This marks it as effective technique which can help cultivate positive mental health in normal individuals27 and be an effective instrument regarding preventive medicine and public health.

Studies suggest that long-term meditation practice can reduce stress reactions, a fall in reduction-oxidation metabolic conditions and could be therapeutically beneficial in chronic inflammatory conditions characterized by neurogenic inflammation28. The cumulative effects of regular mediation practice may, in the long run, help improve the physiochemical conditions where synapses occur and could represent a useful preventive strategy for age-related chronic diseases29. Even so, there is still much to study given the technical limitations and molecular changes which mindfulness practice and conscious breathing could induce, but we may directly infer that metabolic changes improve synaptic tasks and that this improves the task of learning. Meditation and conscious breathing measurements suggest that this metabolic change occurs and that we can infer cognitive improvement.

Conscious breathing and cognitive effects

Studies suggest that mental training, meditation, and other similar tasks may be used as tools in neuroscientific studies of the brain and cognitive plasticity30. The brain changes as a function of experience or learning, dependent on stimuli, creating changes in neuronal pathways,31 and deeper molecular remodeling giving evidence of the phenomenon of metaplasticity32. Therefore, some authors mention in their publications that some cognitive processes and executive functions can be improved with meditation, secondary to synaptic changes. A study was done on adolescents where the participants (N = 198 adolescents) were randomly assigned to mindfulness meditation, hatha yoga, or a control group. The mindfulness meditation group participants showed significant improvements in their working memory, unlike the control and hatha yoga groups33. Other short-term meditation studies show that meditation improves executive functions, including attention and working memory processes. However, they argue that it is unclear how much contextual effects contribute to these improvements34, although we can suggest metabolic changes related to improved synaptic function.

Consequently, meditation training improves performance on tasks evaluating attention, control, and memory function35. To explain these effects, current theoretical models emphasize attention as the central component influenced by meditation36. However, can it impact adult learning enough to be used within teaching-learning processes? Apparently, studies confirm that these individuals improve performance related to sequence repetition, allowing for the implementation of response-based planning, and generating benefits from learning them37.

There is evidence suggesting positive physical and psychological clinical results from meditation. These studies show relief for clinical disorders, including anxiety and depression38, eating disorders, addictions and disorders caused by psychoactive drug use39, stress normalization, arterial pressure and cholesterol levels, and cardiovascular disease prevention40, of how it could improve metabolic states, diminishing reduction-oxidation balance, which has been suggested to modulate synaptic function, with effects on what we consider the base of learning, metaplasticity41. It has also been suggested that its effect is achieved by lowering the intensity of emotional excitement; therefore, the task of meditation not only allows us to infer an improved physicochemical environment where synapses occur, but also to achieve better balance in the autonomous system, allowing us to indicate an improved learning environment. For this reason, since 1996, Varela has spoken about neurophenomenology, regarding consciousness studies where subjective experience and brain dynamics are integrated, allowing for simultaneous measurement of subjective experience (experiential correlations), phenomenological correlations, and neurophysiological correlations for the same time period or event42. In this, the state produces a change in brain activity together with a phenomenological change according to the qualitative research done with people in hypnosis or meditation43.

Electrophysiological and imaging evidence

In 2011, Hölzel, et al. analyzed magnetic resonance images and compared cortical density, before and after meditation training, and found structural changes which are secondary to increased synaptic connections in meditators. These changes are represented by an increased amount of grey matter in the left hippocampus, the posterior cingulate cortex, the temporoparietal union and the cerebellum, all areas involved in learning, memory processing, and emotional regulation44. The increased hippocampal structure may be secondary to the activation of the hippocampus. This change in function could be related to the consolidation of declarative memory long-term memory, such as the recovery of spatial information coding and autobiographical memory45. Researchers have also documented the effects of meditation on spatio-visual memory spatial memory recovery and recognition of objects and codification of distances between reference points46 and the visualization of interior or exterior scenes representing a spatial design47 including the imagination and representation of complex scenes48, also activating the parahippocampal area, this latter effect being mainly due to structural changes, but which as mentioned are secondary to functional modulations.49

Conclusions

Figure 1 shows a summary of our idea, present now, in this review. We suggested some of the cellular efects of conscious breathing. Synaptic activity is modulable at the molecular level, due to a phenomenon known as metaplasticity. Recent evidence indicates that this is an ongoing phenomenon in the brain and accounts for learning in various models. This phenomenon also does not disappear, but rather changes its form of stimulation suggesting that it does not decline in age, but rather adapts to new conditions, Molecular changes occur in appropriate physiochemical conditions and with the right level of attention, especially regulating the autonomous system. The tasks of meditation and breathing have been described as affecting this type of process, improving metabolic parameters and regulating the autonomous system, thereby reducing stress. This could allow us to infer a mechanism carrying out structural changes in relevant brain areas for cognitive task development. These points could suggest that conscious breathing activities improve metabolic processes, allowing for improved synaptic function modulating remodeling culminating in changes in critical brain areas and improved cognitive function. This, in the framework of andragogic learning, could be a new paradigm for working on adult learning, where teaching conscious breathing may be helpful in adults for cognitive tasks in a context of new learning.

Figure 1 The figure is a summary model of our review, where we indicate that the use and development of conscious breathing improves cognitive tasks, enabling better behavior in adults. This can be explained by changes in the physiological conditions in which metaplastic phenomena occur and we can suggest that learning can be enhanced by this type of simple and easy to introduce activity. 

REFERENCES

1. Thompson E, Varela FJ. Radical embodiment:neural dynamics and consciousness. Trends Cogn Sci. 2001;5:418-25. [ Links ]

2. Mosini AC, Saad M, Braghetta CC, Medeiros R, Peres MF, Leao FC. Neurophysiological, cognitive-behavioral and neurochemical effects in practitioners of transcendental meditation - A literature review. Rev Assoc Med Bras (1992). 2019;65:706-13. [ Links ]

3. Voss P, Thomas ME, Cisneros-Franco JM, de Villers-Sidani É. Dynamic brains and the changing rules of neuroplasticity:implications for learning and recovery. Front psychol. 2017;8:1657. [ Links ]

4. Cooke SF, Bliss TV. Plasticity in the human central nervous system. Brain. 2006;129:1659-73. [ Links ]

5. Chavez-Mancilla V, Parodi J. Neurobiological bases of learning and their role for the paradigm shift in education. Psychology. 2015;6:9. [ Links ]

6. Sadaf S, Ali SK, Zuberi RW. Educational models and strategies in health education. Introduction. Educ Health (Abingdon). 2009;22:96. [ Links ]

7. Youde A. Andragogy in blended learning contexts:effective tutoring of adult learners studying part-time, vocationally relevant degrees at a distance. Int J Lifelong Educ. 2018;37:255-72. [ Links ]

8. Wang VC, Storey VA. Andragogy and teaching english as a foreign language in China. Ref Libr. 2015;56:295-314. [ Links ]

9. Watts KA. Tools and principles for effective online library instruction:andragogy and undergraduates. J Libr Inform Serv Distance Learn. 2018;12:49-55. [ Links ]

10. Bergman EM, de Bruin AB, Vorstenbosch MA, Kooloos JG, Puts GC, Leppink J, et al. Effects of learning content in context on knowledge acquisition and recall:a pretest-posttest control group design. BMC Med Educ. 2015;15:133. [ Links ]

11. Hopfinger JB. Introduction to special issue:attention and plasticity. Cogn Neurosci. 2017;8:69-71. [ Links ]

12. Rombouts JO, Bohte SM, Roelfsema PR. How attention can create synaptic tags for the learning of working memories in sequential tasks. PLoS Comput Biol. 2015;11:e1004060. [ Links ]

13. Swingler MM, Perry NB, Calkins SD. Neural plasticity and the development of attention:intrinsic and extrinsic influences. Dev Psychopathol. 2015;27:443-57. [ Links ]

14. Ahissar M, Hochstein S. Attentional control of early perceptual learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1993;90:5718-22. [ Links ]

15. Couperus JW. Implicit learning modulates selective attention at sensory levels of perceptual processing. Atten Percept Psychophys. 2009;71:342-51. [ Links ]

16. Sasaki Y, Nanez JE, Watanabe T. Advances in visual perceptual learning and plasticity. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:53-60. [ Links ]

17. Vartak D, Jeurissen D, Self MW, Roelfsema PR. The influence of attention and reward on the learning of stimulus-response associations. Sci Rep. 2017;7:9036. [ Links ]

18. Salthouse TA. When does age-related cognitive decline begin?Neurobiol Aging. 2009;30:507-14. [ Links ]

19. Raz N, Rodriguez KM. Differential aging of the brain:patterns, cognitive correlates and modifiers. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30:730-48. [ Links ]

20. Ding XQ, Maudsley AA, Sabati M, Sheriff S, Schmitz B, Schutze M. Physiological neuronal decline in healthy aging human brain-an in vivo study with MRI and short echo-time whole-brain (1)H MR spectroscopic imaging. Neuroimage. 2016;137:45-51. [ Links ]

21. Holmes MJ, Robertson FC, Little F, Randall SR, Cotton MF, van der Kouwe AJ, et al. Longitudinal increases of brain metabolite levels in 5-10 year old children. PLoS One. 2017;12:e0180973. [ Links ]

22. Slagter HA, Davidson RJ, Lutz A. Mental training as a tool in the neuroscientific study of brain and cognitive plasticity. Front Hum Neurosci. 2011;5:17. [ Links ]

23. Lutz A, Brefczynski-Lewis J, Johnstone T, Davidson RJ. Regulation of the neural circuitry of emotion by compassion meditation:effects of meditative expertise. PLoS One. 2008;3:e1897. [ Links ]

24. Wielgosz J, Goldberg SB, Kral TR, Dunne JD, Davidson RJ. Mindfulness meditation and psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2019;15:285-316. [ Links ]

25. Dahl CJ, Davidson RJ. Mindfulness and the contemplative life:pathways to connection, insight, and purpose. Curr Opin Psychol. 2019;28:60-4. [ Links ]

26. MacKenzie MB, Abbott KA, Kocovski NL. Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in patients with depression:current perspectives. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2018;14:1599-605. [ Links ]

27. Sanivarapu S. Free your mind:mindfulness practice of Buddhist philosophy. Indian J Psychiatry. 2016;58:345. [ Links ]

28. Rosenkranz MA, Lutz A, Perlman DM, Bachhuber DR, Schuyler BS, MacCoon DG, et al. Reduced stress and inflammatory responsiveness in experienced meditators compared to a matched healthy control group. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2016;68:117-25. [ Links ]

29. Chaix R, Alvarez-Lopez MJ, Fagny M, Lemee L, Regnault B, Davidson RJ, et al. Epigenetic clock analysis in long-term meditators. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2017;85:210-4. [ Links ]

30. Slagter HA, Lutz A, Greischar LL, Francis AD, Nieuwenhuis S, Davis JM, et al. Mental training affects distribution of limited brain resources. PLoS Biol. 2007;5:e138. [ Links ]

31. Lazar SW, Kerr CE, Wasserman RH, Gray JR, Greve DN, Treadway MT, et al. Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness. Neuroreport. 2005;16:1893-7. [ Links ]

32. Yee AX, Hsu YT, Chen L. A metaplasticity view of the interaction between homeostatic and Hebbian plasticity. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2017;372:20160155. [ Links ]

33. Quach D, Jastrowski Mano KE, Alexander K. A randomized controlled trial examining the effect of mindfulness meditation on working memory capacity in adolescents. J Adolesc Health. 2016;58:489-96. [ Links ]

34. Pratzlich M, Kossowsky J, Gaab J, Krummenacher P. Impact of short-term meditation and expectation on executive brain functions. Behav Brain Res. 2016;297:268-76. [ Links ]

35. Gallant SN. Mindfulness meditation practice and executive functioning:breaking down the benefit. Conscious Cogn. 2016;40:116-30. [ Links ]

36. Moore A, Gruber T, Derose J, Malinowski P. Regular, brief mindfulness meditation practice improves electrophysiological markers of attentional control. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012;6:18. [ Links ]

37. Chan RW, Immink MA, Lushington K. The influence of focused-attention meditation states on the cognitive control of sequence learning. Conscious Cogn. 2017;55:11-25. [ Links ]

38. Manuello J, Vercelli U, Nani A, Costa T, Cauda F. Mindfulness meditation and consciousness:an integrative neuroscientific perspective. Conscious Cogn. 2016;40:67-78. [ Links ]

39. Khanna S, Greeson JM. A narrative review of yoga and mindfulness as complementary therapies for addiction. Complement Ther Med. 2013;21:244-52. [ Links ]

40. Anderson JW, Liu C, Kryscio RJ. Blood pressure response to transcendental meditation:a meta-analysis. Am J Hypertens. 2008;21:310-6. [ Links ]

41. Crestani AP, Krueger JN, Barragan EV, Nakazawa Y, Nemes SE, Quillfeldt JA, et al. Metaplasticity contributes to memory formation in the hippocampus. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2019;44:408-14. [ Links ]

42. Varela F. Neurophenomenology. A methodological remedy to the hard problem. J Conscious Stud. 1996;3:330-49. [ Links ]

43. Cardena E. The phenomenology of deep hypnosis:quiescent and physically active. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 2005;53:37-59. [ Links ]

44. Holzel BK, Carmody J, Vangel M, Congleton C, Yerramsetti SM, Gard T, et al. Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional brain gray matter density. Psychiatry Res. 2011;191:36-43. [ Links ]

45. Cipolotti L, Moscovitch M. The hippocampus and remote autobiographical memory. Lancet Neurol. 2005;4:792-3. [ Links ]

46. Tsukiura T, Fujii T, Okuda J, Ohtake H, Kawashima R, Itoh M, et al. Time-dependent contribution of the hippocampal complex when remembering the past:a PET study. Neuroreport. 2002;13:2319-23. [ Links ]

47. O'Craven KM, Kanwisher N. Mental imagery of faces and places activates corresponding stiimulus-specific brain regions. J Cogn Neurosci. 2000;12:1013-23. [ Links ]

48. Hassabis D, Kumaran D, Maguire EA. Using imagination to understand the neural basis of episodic memory. J Neurosci. 2007;27:14365-74. [ Links ]

49. Lv J, Feng H, Chen L, Wang WY, Yue XL, Jin QH. Activation of beta-adrenoceptor facilitates active avoidance learning through enhancement of glutamate levels in the hippocampal dentate gyrus. Neuroreport. 2017;28:973-9. [ Links ]

FundingThis research received the support of Proyecto Interno Universidad Mayor, and Tonalli ltda.

Ethical disclosures

Protection of human and animal subjects. The authors declare that no experiments were performed on humans or animals for this study.

Confidentiality of data. The authors declare that no patient data appear in this article.

Right to privacy and informed consent. The authors declare that no patient data appear in this article.

Received: April 02, 2021; Accepted: May 03, 2021

* Correspondence: Jorge Parodi E-mail: jparodi2010@gmail.com

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interests.

Creative Commons License Instituto Nacional de Cardiología Ignacio Chávez. Published by Permanyer. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license